Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 27 March 2023
Sec. Speech and Language
Volume 17 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2023.1139292

A narrative review of the anatomy and function of the white matter tracts in language production and comprehension

  • 1Department of Neuroscience, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
  • 2Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 3Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition (CNBC), Pittsburgh, PA, United States

Much is known about the role of cortical areas in language processing. The shift towards network approaches in recent years has highlighted the importance of uncovering the role of white matter in connecting these areas. However, despite a large body of research, many of these tracts’ functions are not well-understood. We present a comprehensive review of the empirical evidence on the role of eight major tracts that are hypothesized to be involved in language processing (inferior longitudinal fasciculus, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, uncinate fasciculus, extreme capsule, middle longitudinal fasciculus, superior longitudinal fasciculus, arcuate fasciculus, and frontal aslant tract). For each tract, we hypothesize its role based on the function of the cortical regions it connects. We then evaluate these hypotheses with data from three sources: studies in neurotypical individuals, neuropsychological data, and intraoperative stimulation studies. Finally, we summarize the conclusions supported by the data and highlight the areas needing further investigation.

1. Introduction

Detailed reviews exist of the role of cortical regions in language production and comprehension (e.g., Price, 2012; Kemmerer, 2019; Nozari, 2021). In recent years, however, interest has extended from uncovering the role of gray matter to how the interactions between different cortical regions give rise to language processing. A significant methodological development in this vein has been the study of white matter tracts, i.e., the pathways that connect various bodies of gray matter. The ultimate white matter map, the human connectome, represents a complex network of connections that forms the neurobiological basis of human cognition, including language processing. Compared to the study of gray matter, the study of white matter tracts in language processing is still in its infancy. New tracts are discovered, better anatomical descriptions of known tracts are offered, and new and more nuanced functions for each tract are frequently proposed in recent publications. The purpose of the current article is to present an up-to-date narrative review of the white matter tracts involved in language production and comprehension. We first present an overview of the computational architecture of comprehension and production, followed by a brief review of the role of the cortical regions in carrying out those computations. Next, we focus on each individual tract, its anatomical connections, and its hypothesized role(s) based on the cortical regions it connects. We then review the empirical evidence for and against such hypotheses, summarize the conclusions, and point out areas in need of further research.

2. The computational architecture of language production and comprehension

Years of research and a large body of empirical evidence have been dedicated to uncovering the nature and levels of representations in language production and comprehension and the principles that govern these systems, leading to the proposal of sophisticated computational models (e.g., Dell, 1986; McClelland and Elman, 1986; Levelt et al., 1999). The gist is that the two systems have much in common (Figure 1). Production starts with formulating a message through the activation of semantic knowledge, and continues by activating lexical items, ordering them into a syntactic sequence, mapping each word onto its phonemes, activating the articulatory phonetic representations corresponding to the phonological plans, and ultimately executing speech motor commands. The system has a number of key properties: (1) spreading activation not only activates the target (e.g., cat) but also related representations (e.g., “dog”; Levelt et al., 1999; see Nozari and Pinet, 2020, for a review). (2) Activation is cascaded, meaning that activated non-target representations (e.g., “dog”) also activate their segments (e.g., /d/; Dell, 1986). (3) The system has some degree of feedback from later to earlier layers, i.e., phonemes /æ/ and /t/ in “cat” feedback to other words that share them (e.g., “mat”) and activate them (Dell, 1986; Rapp and Goldrick, 2000). These general properties are observed not only in spoken production but also in other production modalities such as handwriting and typing (e.g., Rapp and Fischer-Baum, 2014; Pinet and Nozari, 2018).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A schematic of the cognitive architecture of language production and comprehension. Depending on the input and direction of processing, different task architectures can be identified with this figure. For example, the central bubble, visual objects, shows the starting point of picture naming, which ends in motor output. The left bubble shows the starting point of auditory word repetition, which can be lexical or sublexical, depending on whether it activates words or phonemes, respectively. The right bubble shows the starting point of reading with visual orthographic input. Reading can also be carried out by directly activating words or through orthographic-phonological conversion. Some details are omitted to focus on showing how different task structures overlap in the language system.

In many ways, comprehension can be viewed as an inverted version of production (see Figure 1). Here, the acoustic signal first activates the phonetic features. These features then activate phonemes, words, and ultimately semantic knowledge, translating sound into a meaningful message. While the nature of lower-level representations (articulatory-phonetic features vs. acoustic features) obviously differs between production and comprehension, most researchers agree that higher-level representations, e.g., words, semantic features, and syntactic structures are shared between the two (e.g., Warker et al., 2009; Nozari, 2020). Moreover, similar to production, comprehension also involves the co-activation of related non-target representations, cascading, and feedback (McClelland and Elman, 1986). These properties have several consequences for the studies of the neurobiology of language. Isolating various components (e.g., word representations) in cascaded systems is not easy. This is because activation can rapidly spread through the later layers of the system (e.g., Costa et al., 2009) while still converging on specific representations in earlier layers. The feedback from later to earlier layers further complicates the interpretation of events using a linear timeline. This, in turn, leads to difficulty in separating operations such as semantic-lexical activation and lexical selection (Riès et al., 2017). The good news is that despite the characteristics of cascading and interactivity, the evidence shows that, generally speaking, semantic-to-lexical mapping occurs earlier than lexical-to-phonological mapping (Dell, 1986; Rapp and Goldrick, 2000; Pinet and Nozari, 2023; see Dell et al., 2014, for a review). This so-called global modularity, despite local interactivity, has been a key factor in the success of neural studies in pinpointing individual operations to specific neural regions, but it is important to keep in mind that a clean demarcation between operations such as lexical activation and lexical selection and the neural regions responsible for the two is unlikely to be possible (Riès et al., 2017).

3. Neural underpinnings of language production and comprehension

Early studies of the neurobiology of language were mostly focused on defining the specific role of various cortical regions in language production and comprehension. This research has been largely successful in reconstructing the language network. The most widely accepted version of this network is Hickok and Poeppel’s (2007) dual-stream network. In this model, a largely bilaterally organized ventral stream is responsible for mapping sound to meaning. On the other hand, a predominantly left-lateralized dorsal stream maps the acoustic signal to articulatory motor commands. The two streams, thus, roughly carry out the operations related to comprehension and production, although equating the dorsal stream with production emphasizes production tasks that start with an available phonological sequence (e.g., auditory repetition). Production attempts that start from meaning (as most real-life conversations do) are likely to also involve a large portion of the ventral stream which carries out semantic-lexical processing. Relatedly, comprehension may entail production components, e.g., in the form of subvocal articulation (e.g., Price, 2010), making the contributions of the two streams to comprehension and production less modular. With that in mind, we briefly review the role proposed for different cortical regions in these two streams for language production and comprehension. In later sections, we will use this information to generate predictions about the role of white matter tracts connecting these cortical regions.

3.1. Semantic-lexical processing

Semantic processing is common to both comprehension and production, and as mentioned earlier, is unlikely to contain duplicate representations for these two systems. Detailed reviews of the semantic network exist elsewhere (e.g., Binder, 2007), but the gist is that there is an extensive network of distributed features (many of which are in the sensory-motor cortex) with potential “hubs” or convergent zones which represent unified concepts (Patterson and Lambon Ralph, 2016). There is disagreement about the degree to which such hubs contain lexical information vs. pure conceptual knowledge (e.g., Kemmerer, 2019), but computationally speaking, such hubs represent a graded translation of a massive, distributed network of semantic features into a much smaller space of phonological forms. Anterior and middle parts of the lateral temporal cortex (and less often the inferior temporal gyrus; ITG) have been the prime candidates for containing these semantic-lexical representations (Indefrey and Levelt, 2004; Binder et al., 2009; Walker et al., 2011). Of the two, the anterior temporal lobe (ATL) has been more strongly linked to semantic and the middle temporal gyrus (MTG) to lexical processing (Hickok and Poeppel, 2004; Indefrey and Levelt, 2004; Visser et al., 2010). In addition to the temporal cortex, parietal regions, especially the angular gyrus (AG), have been implicated in semantic processing (Binder et al., 2009; Price et al., 2015). However, unlike the temporal regions that largely represent individual objects and concepts, the parietal regions appear to be involved in integrative semantic processing, such as event representation (e.g., Binder and Desai, 2011; Ramanan et al., 2018). Finally, frontal regions are often activated during tasks that require semantic-lexical processing. This activation has been taken as representing a top-down boost, either for strengthening associations or for resolving conflict between competing alternatives (e.g., Thompson-Schill et al., 1997, 1998; Wagner et al., 2001). Both point to the concept of “semantic control” (as opposed to the simple activation of semantic knowledge) and mark the critical importance of the connections between temporo-parietal and frontal regions for semantic-lexical processing.

3.2. Processing of the acoustic signal

This function is primarily related to comprehension, although it also plays a role in regulating production through monitoring (e.g., Guenther, 2016). The regions usually implicated in the processing of the acoustic signal are the superior temporal gyrus (STG), Heschel’s gyrus, and the superior temporal sulcus (STS; e.g., Hickok, 2012). However, tasks that entail auditory discrimination such as changes to the phonetic category may also tap into other regions, such as the left dorsal pars opercularis in the IFG (e.g., Blumstein et al., 2005), indicating the importance of connections between the temporal auditory cortex and other regions.

3.3. Phonological processing

This function is hypothesized to be common to both production and comprehension, although not as uncontroversially as semantic-lexical processing. For example, while some researchers posit the existence of phonemes as distinct representations in perception (e.g., Hickok, 2012), others have questioned this assumption (e.g., Samuel, 2020). When assumed to be independent representations, the neural correlates of phonological processing have often been pinpointed to the posterior STG (pSTG), supramarginal gyrus (SMG), and sometimes posterior MTG (pMTG; e.g., Schwartz et al., 2012; Binder, 2015). It is noteworthy that phonological processing is often confounded with operations underlying phonological working memory (PWM), because keeping a phonological sequence active, say to output in production, relies on PWM. The latter is often localized to the inferior parietal cortex, especially SMG (e.g., Yue et al., 2018), and sometimes extends to the planum temporale (e.g., Buchsbaum and D’Esposito, 2008), although a frontal component has also been identified, which is hypothesized to mark the verbal rehearsal strategy related to keeping phonological forms active in working memory (e.g., Baldo and Dronkers, 2006).

3.4. Articulatory processing

This process is primarily related to speech production, although it is sometimes seen during comprehension as well (Price, 2010). The goal of this operation is to translate the phonological representations into motor commands. The neural regions proposed for this operation are the lateral and medial surfaces of the frontal cortex. GODIVA (see Guenther, 2016, for a history and complete review) is the most complete model of motor speech production and divides the process into a planning loop and a motor loop. The planning loop consists of the pre-supplementary motor area (preSMA) and left posterior inferior frontal sulcus (pIFS), and temporarily buffers the utterance before articulation. The motor loop consists of the supplementary motor area (SMA) and the ventral premotor cortex (vPMC) and executes the articulatory motor commands. A combined signal from the SMA and vPMC activates motor gestures in the ventral motor cortex (vMC), which drives the articulators (Guenther, 2016; Nozari, 2021).

3.5. Syntactic processing

The operations reviewed above are all involved in single-word processing, but speech often consists of phrases, sentences, and paragraphs. Although the body of literature probing the neural correlates of syntactic production is not small, pinpointing the neural substrates of syntax has been far from easy. For years, the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), especially pars triangularis, was considered the main region involved in syntactic processing (Grodzinsky and Santi, 2008; Hagoort, 2014; Friederici, 2017; Matchin et al., 2017), primarily based on the neuropsychological evidence of patients with Broca’s aphasia suffering from agrammatism (Goodglass et al., 1968; Caramazza and Zurif, 1976; Goodglass, 1993). In line with this proposal, several high-powered lesion-symptom mapping studies linked IFG lesions to syntactic parsing deficits in comprehension (Wilson et al., 2010, 2011; Magnusdottir et al., 2013; Mesulam et al., 2015; Fridriksson et al., 2018). These were complemented with neuroimaging studies linking syntactic comprehension to IFG (Friederici, 2011, 2017; Hagoort, 2014). At the same time, more and more studies pointed out an even more prominent link between syntactic processing deficits and regions in the posterior temporal cortex (Dronkers et al., 2004; Wilson and Saygın, 2004; Baldo and Dronkers, 2007; Peelle et al., 2008; Pillay et al., 2017; Rogalsky et al., 2018; Wilson et al., 2018). In reviewing the neuroimaging data linking IFG to syntactic comprehension, Matchin et al. (2017) point out that the activation of IFG is almost always observed along with that of the posterior temporal lobe. The distinction is further complicated by the proposed involvement of the IFG in working memory and executive control processes that syntactic processing, in most cases, taps into (e.g., Rogalsky and Hickok, 2011; Nozari and Thompson-Schill, 2013, 2016; Nozari et al., 2014a, b; Arnold and Nozari, 2017). For this reason, some have proposed the posterior temporal cortex as a more critical region in syntactic processing than the IFG (Bornkessel-Schlesewsky et al., 2015; Pillay et al., 2017). A more nuanced proposal has been recently put forth by Matchin et al. (2017). The proposal emphasizes the different computational demands of syntactic processing in comprehension and production, which give rise to differential predictions regarding the role of certain regions in syntactic processing depending on the task. Specifically, Matchin et al. (2017) propose that in comprehension, auditory sequences in pSTG are decoded into hierarchical structures in pMTG, and are further connected to two semantic hubs, the ATL and the AG, representing the knowledge of objects and events, respectively (Binder and Desai, 2011). In production, the unstructured semantic information is turned into hierarchical propositions by the pMTG and passed on to the IFG’s pars triangularis for conversion into morphological chunks.

4. White matter tracts involved in language processing

As implied by their names, ventral and dorsal “streams” are more than just a collection of disconnected cortical regions. Rather, they mark connected pathways involved in semantic-lexical and phonological-motor processing, respectively. This rough division is a useful guide for identifying the white matter tracts potentially involved in language processing, although researchers sometimes differ in their assignment of tracts to streams, especially for multi-branch tracts that may encompass both streams. Generally speaking, the STG and Sylvian fissure mark the horizontal boundary between the ventral and dorsal streams (Hickok and Poeppel, 2007; Weiller et al., 2021). The ventral stream is often thought to include the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), the uncinate fasciculus (UF), the extreme capsule (EmC), and a branch of the middle longitudinal fasciculus (MdLF; Saur et al., 2008; Wong et al., 2011; Dick and Tremblay, 2012; Yang et al., 2017; Weiller et al., 2021). The dorsal stream contains the bulk of SLF, consisting of SLF-I, SLF-II, SLF-III, and SLF-tp, a part of the SLF that connects temporal and parietal lobes. Some researchers also consider the arcuate fasciculus (AF) to be another branch of the SLF. Finally, a more recently discovered tract, the frontal aslant tract (FAT; Catani et al., 2012, 2013), lies in the anteriormost part of the dorsal tract. In addition to these, there are a few other small tracts that are not frequently included in studies of white matter for language, such as the operculo-premotor fascicle (OpPMF) connecting the pars opercularis to the premotor region and trianguloorbitaris system (TrOrS) connecting the pars triangularis to the pars orbitalis. These tracts are usually difficult to identify in fiber-tracking studies because of their small size and their overlap with SLF III (Lemaire et al., 2013). Although some have suggested a role of these tracts in language processing based on their anatomical connections (Lemaire et al., 2013; Mandonnet et al., 2016), functional data on these tracts are currently sparse. Therefore, we do not include them in this article.

In what follows, we discuss the above-mentioned tracts individually (or sometimes in pairs for comparison). We first review the anatomy of the tract and the cortical regions it connects, based on which predictions about its function can be generated. We then review the empirical evidence regarding the role of the tract, with a heavier focus on its involvement in language processing, and discuss the extent to which the current evidence supports the predictions.

4.1. Methodological preview

Empirical evidence for studying white matter connectivity comes from several different sources. A precise method for studying the anatomy of white matter, used by early anatomists, is post-mortem fiber dissection. This technique entails the peeling of the white matter tracts from the brain and displaying their 3-dimensional structure. The complex and cumbersome procedures required for the preparation of the brain tissue for fiber dissection, together with the emergence of non-invasive methods, have decreased the popularity of this approach, although its precision for studying the subcomponents of white matter tracts has led to renewed interest in its revival in recent years (Martino et al., 2013; Kalyvas et al., 2020). Another precise method is autoradiography, an imaging technique using radioactive tracers, which allows for clear tracing of the origins and termination points of neural pathways (Cowan et al., 1972). Due to toxicity, in vivo autoradiography is not an option in humans but has been a widely used technique for the identification of white matter pathways in primates, such as Rhesus monkeys (Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006; Schmahmann et al., 2007).

Although precise, neither of the two methods described above is practical for studying white matter structures in living humans. A much more popular and widely used non-invasive technique for analyzing white matter in humans is diffusion MRI (dMRI). The most frequent method for analyzing the data is diffusion tensor imaging (DTI; Basser et al., 1994; Mori and Zhang, 2006; Mukherjee et al., 2008; Craddock et al., 2013). This technique relies on the displacement of water molecules in the tissue (Basser et al., 1994). The anisotropic nature of water molecules forms the basis of the quantitative DTI measures: while water molecules diffuse more freely along the axons, myelin sheaths restrict the diffusion of molecules perpendicular to the axonal lines. This difference in the diffusion rate can be used to reconstruct the white matter architecture (Basser et al., 1994; Le Bihan et al., 2001; Alexander et al., 2007). DTI is informative in uncovering the white matter structure in both neurotypical individuals and clinical populations. It uses a number of metrics, the most common of which are fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD), radial diffusivity (RD), and axial diffusivity (AD). FA varies between 0 and 1 and measures the degree of diffusion anisotropy. When diffusion is unrestricted (or equally restricted in all directions), FA is 0. When, on the other hand, diffusion is fully restricted along one axis, FA is 1. Therefore, in gray matter in which the motion of molecules is in all directions, FA is low. In the white matter, the perpendicular motion of molecules is restricted by the myelin sheath and therefore diffusion is anisotropic and FA approaches 1 (Le Bihan et al., 2001; Alexander et al., 2007). Mean diffusivity (MD) measures the overall diffusivity in the tissue. Similar to FA, it is sensitive to the barriers of diffusivity but, unlike FA, it is insensitive to the direction of the diffusion, i.e., it measures the rotationally invariant magnitude of water diffusion in the tissue (Le Bihan et al., 2001; Alexander et al., 2007). In case, of lesions to the white matter, MD increases (Madden et al., 2012). AD and RD indicate the direction of diffusivity. AD describes the magnitude of diffusion parallel to axons, and is a specific marker of axonal degeneration, whereas RD describes the diffusivity perpendicular to axonal fibers and is more sensitive to demyelination (Le Bihan et al., 2001; Alexander et al., 2007). Thus, different measures may be useful in detecting different deficits. For example, relatively pure myelin deficits that are undetectable with FA, often lead to a modest increase in RD (De Erausquin and Alba-Ferrara, 2013). It is important to note that DTI is a correlational technique, meaning that although an association can be established between certain structural properties of the white matter tracts and behavior, it may not translate into a causal role of the tract in generating a certain behavior. Nevertheless, gaining insight into the structural connectivity by DTI, combined with resting-state functional connectivity by fMRI could improve our understanding of network structures underlying brain functions (Skudlarski et al., 2008; Greicius et al., 2009).

Another popular technique for studying white matter is voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VLSM) or its related technique voxel-based morphometry (VBM; Bates et al., 2003; Mechelli et al., 2005). These techniques are heavily employed in neuropsychological work and define a statistical relationship between lesion (measured in voxels) and behavioral impairment (Harvey and Schnur, 2015; Gleichgerrcht et al., 2017; Faulkner and Wilshire, 2020). The rationale is that if damage to a tract leads to an impairment in behavior, the tract is likely to play a critical role in carrying out that behavior. Although the results are less prone to interpretation as an epiphenomenon compared to associative measures, it is still difficult to pinpoint a function to a single tract. The reason is that impaired behavior as a function of a lesioned tract may reflect the disruption of a network, another part of which may play an even more critical role in the behavior than the lesioned tract itself.

Unlike DTI and VLSM/VBM, direct stimulation of white matter (DES; Mandonnet et al., 2010; Duffau et al., 2014; Duffau, 2015) is an invasive methodology that allows for real-time direct functional mapping of white matter tracts intraoperatively. It involves applying localized electrical stimulation to cortical and/or subcortical areas via either a monopolar or bipolar electrode to produce a transient inhibition or excitation in function. The technique may be employed in both an asleep and awake patient. In an asleep patient, where eliciting an overt behavioral response is not possible, electrophysiological measures such as electromyograms or visually evoked potentials are sometimes obtained. In awake patients, changes to performance on a behavioral task is often measured as a function of stimulation. Since the technique involves direct manipulation of the tissue, a change in behavior can be more readily translated into a causal role for the tissue in generating that behavior. Finally, we include some developmental studies in our review. It is important to note that the network for a cognitive function may change over the course of development, but including developmental data in the review allows us to examine whether a certain tract has been implicated in learning a function, even if after learning the function may continue to operate without strong dependence on that tract.

The techniques named above have identified three main types of white matter pathways: projection, commissural, and association fibers (Gottlieb and Cowan, 1973; Schmahmann et al., 2007; Wedeen et al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2010). Projection fibers are ascending and descending fibers which connect the cortex with the brainstem, cerebellum, and spinal cord. The most well-known projection fiber in the brain is the internal capsule. The commissural fibers are axons that connect the two hemispheres (Catani et al., 2002). The main commissural fibers are the corpus callosum, the anterior commissure, and the posterior commissure. Association fibers are axons which connect cortical areas within the same hemisphere. Long and short association fibers connect distant and adjacent areas, respectively (Guevara et al., 2020). Here, we focus on the role of association fibers in language processing.

5. ILF and IFOF

5.1. Anatomy

The ILF and IFOF are the two major fiber tracts connecting the occipital lobe to the anterior regions (temporal and frontal lobes; Figures 2A–F). After some initial controversies about the nature of these pathways, DTI, electrostimulation, and non-human primate studies have now established that ILF and IFOF are long association—and not projection—fibers (Mettler, 1935; Seltzer and Pandya, 1984; Catani et al., 2002, 2003; Wakana et al., 2004; Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006; Mandonnet et al., 2007; Hua et al., 2008; Oishi et al., 2011). There has also been much debate on whether ILF and IFOF are indeed two separate tracts or whether IFOF is the continuation of ILF into the MdLF, EmC, and UF (Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006). This debate is important, in part because of the different endpoints of these tracts: ILF ends in the temporal pole, whereas IFOF ends in the frontal cortex. Therefore, the existence of IFOF as an independent tract would point to direct connections between the occipital and frontal lobes. Unlike studies of non-human primates (Mettler, 1935; Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006; Yeterian et al., 2012), DTI studies in humans consistently support the separation of these two tracts (Catani et al., 2002, 2003; Wakana et al., 2007; Hua et al., 2008; Oishi et al., 2010; Holl et al., 2011; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2011; Turken and Dronkers, 2011; Caverzasi et al., 2014). Given our focus on the role of these tracts in human cognition, we will follow the DTI findings and assume that they are separate tracts, but discuss them together to compare and contrast when possible.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Anatomy of ILF (A: schematic; C: tractography, axial view; D: tractography, sagittal view) and IFOF (B: schematic; E: tractography, axial view; F: tractography, sagittal view). All tractography images have been extracted in the following way: DTI data of a 45-year-old neurotypical male were used for fiber-tracking of the language tracts. ROIs were defined automatically based on the anatomical atlas loaded into the DSI studio program. The angular threshold was 90 degrees. The step size was 0.98 mm and 10,000 tracks were placed. Fiber trajectories were smoothed by averaging the propagation direction of the 30% of previous directions, and tracts shorter than 20 mm were discarded. DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; FA, fractional anisotropy; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; MOG, middle occipital gyrus; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; Ptri, pars triangularis; Porb, pars orbitalis; SOG, superior occipital gyrus; SPL, superior parietal lobe; STG, superior temporal gyrus.

The ILF (Figures 2A,C,D) is a large multilayer fiber tract connecting the occipital cortex with the anterior temporal lobe (Catani et al., 2002, 2003; Panesar et al., 2018; Sali et al., 2018; Zemmoura et al., 2021). Its dorsal component originates from the cuneus and projects to the superior and middle temporal gyri. Its ventral component originates from the lingual and fusiform gyri, and projects to the superior, middle, and inferior temporal gyri (Latini et al., 2017; Panesar et al., 2018; Sali et al., 2018; Zemmoura et al., 2021). The IFOF (Figures 2B,E,F) is also a large multilayered tract originating from the occipital and occipitotemporal, and parietal regions. It runs through the extreme and external capsules and terminates in anterior frontal areas, including the IFG and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex or DLPFC (Duffau, 2015; Conner et al., 2018a). The IFOF has two components: (i) a superficial dorsal component, which connects the pars triangularis and orbitalis with the superior parietal lobe and the posterior portion of the superior and middle occipital gyri; and (ii) a deep ventral component, which connects the posterior portion of the inferior occipital gyrus and the posterior basal temporal region with three different areas in the middle frontal gyrus (MFG), the DLPFC and the orbitofrontal cortex (Martino et al., 2010; Sarubbo et al., 2013; but see Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2016).

5.2. Function

Given the occipital origin of both tracts and the links they provide to temporal, parietal, and frontal regions, it is hardly surprising that they have both been implicated in operations that depend on visual perception, such as object and face recognition and spatial attention. In keeping with these predictions, DTI studies have shown a role for ILF in object recognition (e.g., Mandonnet et al., 2009; Ortibus et al., 2012), scene recognition (e.g., Tavor et al., 2014), and face recognition (e.g., Tavor et al., 2014; Hodgetts et al., 2015). Complementing these, are reports of prosopagnosia after the disruption of right ILF (e.g., Thomas et al., 2009; Valdés-Sosa et al., 2011; Grossi et al., 2014), and the induction of visual hemiagnosia after the stimulation of the right ILF (Coello et al., 2013), further pointing to a critical role of the ILF in face and object recognition.

Evidence for the role of IFOF in face perception is mixed. Some have linked the integrity of IFOF to face perception in prosopagnosia and neurotypical adults (e.g., Thomas et al., 2008; Valdés-Sosa et al., 2011) but others have explicitly shown that such a relationship is limited to the right ILF (e.g., Scherf et al., 2014). In contrast, and perhaps due to its stronger connections with the parietal regions, IFOF is more heavily implicated in spatial attention. For example, a decreased FA in the right IFOF in stroke survivors is associated with visual neglect (Urbanski et al., 2011; Toba et al., 2018). Similarly, Herbet et al. (2017a) showed that subcortical stimulation of right IFOF in glioma patients can cause spatial neglect. Both ILF and IFOF have also been linked to the processing of facial emotions (Baggio et al., 2012; Multani et al., 2017).

5.3. Links to language

Aside from a general role in visually guided behavior, connections to a specific part of the temporal cortex, the visual word form area (VWFA), suggest a potential role of these tracts in a specific language task, i.e., reading and perhaps writing. ILF has been identified as one of the three major tracts associated with the VWFA, with SLF and the vertical occipital fasciculus (vOF) as the other two (Chen et al., 2019). vOF is a fiber bundle connecting dorsolateral and ventrolateral visual cortices but is rarely mentioned in studies of the matter tracts, most likely because of its overlap with the dorsal parts of ILF and IFOF (Yeatman et al., 2014). It is thus important to keep in mind that some of the functions attributed to ILF and IFOF in reading may actually be carried out by vOF. For example, studies that dissociate the vOF from the ILF and IFOF showed that this tract is related to literacy development in children (Broce et al., 2019; Grotheer et al., 2021).

Various studies have linked reading or reading deficits to ILF, IFOF, or both (Horowitz-Kraus et al., 2014; Sarubbo et al., 2015; Vandermosten et al., 2015; Zhao et al., 2016; Arrington et al., 2017; Broce et al., 2019; Kumar and Padakannaya, 2019; Grotheer et al., 2021). For example, after controlling for several factors, including age, gender, IQ, the overall development of the white matter, and phonological skills, Broce et al. (2019) showed that the properties of the left ILF and right IFOF were predictive of early literacy skills in 5–8-year-old children. The left ILF was, however, strongly associated with phonological awareness. In a study comparing typically developing children with children with dyslexia, Zhao et al. (2016) showed that IFOF was significantly less left-lateralized in the dyslexic group, and the degree of lateralization was correlated with reading abilities (see also Steinbrink et al., 2008; Carter et al., 2009; Yeatman et al., 2012; Vandermosten et al., 2015; Su et al., 2018a; Vanderauwera et al., 2018; see Vandermosten et al., 2012 for a review).

The Left ILF’s critical role in orthographic processing was demonstrated in a study of 67 individuals with brain damage, in whom a PCA-derived orthographic index (after regressing out non-orthographic tasks) correlated significantly with left ILF’s integrity, even after controlling for other confounding factors (Wang et al., 2020; see also Su et al., 2018b; Farah et al., 2020). Reports from awake craniotomy studies also confirm the link between these tracts and reading/writing abilities. In a study of glioma patients undergoing awake surgery, Sarubbo et al. (2015) found a close correspondence between the spatial distribution of alexia and that of the ILF (see also Epelbaum et al., 2008; Gil-Robles et al., 2013; Enatsu et al., 2017 for demonstrations of ILF stimulation disturbing reading). Similarly, in a patient undergoing surgery for left inferior parietal glioma, Motomura et al. (2014) applied subcortical stimulation to IFOF and induced transient alexia and agraphia, suggesting a critical role of IFOF in these operations.

The literature above clearly links the ILF and IFOF to language processing through reading. There is, however, evidence that these tracts contribute to language processing in more fundamental ways, namely through their involvement in semantic-lexical processing. General evidence for this claim comes from studies linking comprehension abilities to the ILF, IFOF, or both (e.g., Del Tufo et al., 2019), or demonstrating their abnormalities in impaired semantic processing in neuropsychological disorders (e.g., Whitwell et al., 2010; Botha et al., 2015; D’Anna et al., 2016; Ivanova et al., 2016; Surbeck et al., 2020). There have also been attempts at pinpointing the function(s) of these tracts more precisely. Anatomically, there are two reasons to expect a contribution of these tracts to lexical semantic processing: (a) traversing through the length of the temporal lobe, they connect areas that are clearly implicated in storing conceptual, lexical, and auditory representations, and might thus point to a role in semantic and lexical retrieval. (b) Connections between the temporal lobe (especially MTG) and frontal (and some parietal) regions point to a potential role in semantic and lexical control. The main difference between the two is that the former includes any situations that require activation of semantic-lexical concepts, while the latter selectively involves situations that include either high competition or weak association (Martin-Chang and Levy, 2006; Nozari et al., 2016a). We will return to this point at the end of this section.

The standard test of pure semantic processing is usually a test such as the Pyramids and Palm Trees Test which assesses semantic relations without the need to appeal to lexical labels. There are indeed reports of a correlation between scores on such tests and IFOF in particular (de Zubicaray et al., 2011; Moritz-Gasser et al., 2013; Mirman et al., 2015; Herbet et al., 2017b). But the task is heavily influenced by visual processing abilities that are also linked to ILF and IFOF. To ensure the pure contribution of these tracts to semantic processing independently of visual processing, it is important to use other modalities (e.g., the auditory modality). This often means that some of the tasks entail lexical items, which makes the extraction of pure semantics difficult. Fortunately, based on the earlier discussion of the stages of production, such a clear-cut distinction is perhaps neither necessary nor extremely useful in understanding the neurobiology of language: lexical-semantic representations are representations necessary for the mapping of distributed semantic features onto phonological representations, and likely include multiple layers of representations that gradually move from unifying semantic features to representing the more formal aspects of concepts. We will thus focus on studies providing converging evidence from multiple tasks on the involvement of the ILF/IFOF in lexical-semantic processing.

Faulkner and Wilshire (2020) used VLSM in 63 postoperative tumor patients and found a correlation between lesions in the territory of left ILF and semantic-lexical mapping, which they computed as a function of semantic and omission errors in picture naming, as well as performance on a category probe task after regressing out phonological fluency scores (note that the use of probabilistic maps in that study makes disentangling from close tracts such as left IFOF very difficult). Using diffusion-weighted imaging, Jarret et al. (2022) studied picture naming in 37 neurotypical adults and reported IFOF as a direct pathway for lexicosemantic processing, while ILF and UF were reported as indirect pathways for the same function. In a stud of 55 right-handed older adults, de Zubicaray et al. (2011) used PCA to extract what they referred to as a “semantic memory” component, based on six cognitive tests (Pyramids and Palm Trees, Boston naming, PALPA sentence–picture matching, Category fluency, WAIS-III information, and WAIS-III similarities). They then demonstrated a correlation between this measure and FA values in left IFOF and UF. In a VLSM study of 43 individuals with chronic left-hemisphere stroke, Griffis et al. (2017) showed that lesions in the ILF and IFOF (as well as AF) were associated with poorer performance in picture naming. This may be interpreted as a problem in extracting the visual semantic information, but these lesions were also predictive of poor verbal fluency and auditory semantic decisions, two tasks that do not require visual processing, but instead, semantic-lexical processing. In another large-scale VLSM study of 99 individuals with chronic stroke, Mirman et al. (2015) used a large battery of cognitive tests and used factor analysis to extract three main factors: semantic recognition (encompassing both verbal and non-verbal comprehension), speech recognition (auditory processing of verbal materials), and speech production (phonological and articulatory phonetic encoding). Of the three, semantic recognition was associated with lesions in the IFOF (as well as UF and anterior thalamic radiation or ATR). In another large-scale study of 76 individuals with brain damage, Han et al. (2013) used visual and auditory variants of naming and semantic judgment tasks and found impaired performance to correlate with the FA value of IFOF, as well as the left ATR and UF, after controlling for lesion volume and refuting the influence of gray matter, non-semantic operations (e.g., oral repetition) and numerical cognition.

Together, these studies demonstrate that these tracts play a role in semantic-lexical processing, distinct from other aspects of language processing, such as processing phonological information. The involvement of ILF and IFOF in semantic-lexical processing naturally predicts a role for them in language production from meaning. In line with this prediction, Grossman et al. (2013) studied 15 individuals with nonfluent aphasia and reported a correspondence between the integrity of the fronto-occipital white matter (most likely the IFOF), as well as the UF, and the mean length and well-formedness of utterances. Several studies have more sharply focused on the relationship between these tracts and speech errors in picture naming. If these tracts play a critical role in mapping semantics to lexical items during word production, their disruption should lead to an increase in certain error types but not others. Specifically, we would expect an increase in lexical errors (including semantically related and unrelated words) and possibly omissions (due to the inability to retrieve the correct semantic concept or lexical item). The empirical data are aligned with this prediction: in a study of 32 participants with chronic post-stroke aphasia, McKinnon et al. (2018) found a close correspondence between the number of semantic paraphasias and axonal loss in the ILF. Similarly, in a combined navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation (nTMS) and DTI of 10 patients with brain tumors, Raffa et al. (2016) found a link between semantic errors in picture naming and the ILF and IFOF (see Sarubbo et al., 2013 for a similar link between semantic paraphasias and IFOF). Finally, Stark et al. (2019) studied speech errors of 120 individuals with chronic left-hemisphere stroke in a picture naming task, and also administered a test of connected speech using picture description. In both picture naming and connected speech, unrelated errors were linked to the ILF and IFOF, and in connected speech, semantic paraphasias were also linked to these two tracts (it is noteworthy that SLF was implicated in all of these cases as well).

Complementing these, are studies of intraoperative stimulation of the ILF/IFOF. The majority of such studies have found IFOF stimulation to cause semantic paraphasias (Duffau et al., 2003, 2008, Duffau et al., 2009; Mandonnet et al., 2007; Gil-Robles et al., 2013; Moritz-Gasser et al., 2013; Motomura et al., 2014, 2018; Almairac et al., 2015; Duffau, 2015; Fernández et al., 2020). ILF’s role is more contested; some have found ILF stimulation to cause omission errors (Herbet et al., 2019) and some have found no adverse effect on language production when stimulating this tract (e.g., Mandonnet et al., 2007; Gil-Robles et al., 2013; Moritz-Gasser et al., 2013). Collectively, the body of literature reviewed above supports the involvement of these tracts in semantic-lexical processing, with a suggestion that the ILF may be more strongly involved in lexical-to-semantic mapping (i.e., comprehension) and IFOF in semantic-to-lexical mapping (i.e., production). This distinction was supported in a study by Harvey and Schnur (2015). There are, however, other studies that have attributed comprehension vs. production functions to different parts of each tract. For example, some studies have claimed that the middle and posterior parts of the ILF and IFOF are related to comprehension (Ivanova et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2018), whereas anterior portions of these tracts are important for speech production (Ivanova et al., 2016; Tuncer et al., 2021). Focusing on disentangling the comprehension/production contribution of ILF and IFOF is a fruitful area for future research.

More recent studies have started to tap more closely into the functions of ILF and IFOF in terms of semantic-lexical control. Control has been discussed in two senses in the language literature: conflict and weak associations (e.g., Thompson-Schill and Botvinick, 2006; Martin-Chang and Levy, 2006; see Nozari et al., 2016a for a unifying view). For example, words belonging to the same semantic category but with opposite valences can make semantic judgment harder by inducing conflict. Similarly, words with distant relationships are harder to judge as related, because of the weak association between them. Dávolos et al. (2020) presented participants with visual cue words, followed by three words, from which to choose the most related word to the cue word. Congruency and strength of association were manipulated between the cue and target words and were shown to be correlated with FA in the left ILF, suggesting a role of this tract in lexical-semantic control. FA in the right IFOF, on the other hand, was related to processing under low demands (i.e., no conflict, strong association). In another study, Harvey and Schnur (2015) compared semantic interference in production (using the blocked cyclic naming task) and comprehension (using a word-to-picture matching task) in 18 participants with left hemisphere stroke. They reported an association between the ILF and semantic interference in word comprehension, whereas the IFOF was critical for resolving semantic interference in production. Finally, Nugiel et al. (2016) used a verb generation task in which verbs were elicited from neurotypical individuals either by nouns that strongly elicit a certain verb (e.g., scissors → cut) or by nouns that are loosely associated with multiple verbs (e.g., ball → throw, roll, play, etc.), with the latter requiring greater semantic-lexical control. They found both ILF and IFOF’s structure to be predictive of semantic control in this study. This brief review shows that there is growing evidence for the critical contribution of these two tracts not just to semantic retrieval, but specifically to semantic control, although it remains debated whether the tracts contribute differently to semantic control in comprehension vs. production.

Lastly, we must also mention verbal fluency, as a test commonly used in assessing the contribution of white matter to language production. Verbal fluency tasks come in two varieties: semantic or category fluency refers to a test in which participants are prompted to produce as many words as possible from a certain semantic category (e.g., animals). Phonemic or letter fluency tasks, on the other hand, require participants to produce as many words beginning with a certain sound or letter, irrespective of their semantic category. The idea is that the two variants index semantic-lexical and lexical-segmental processing, respectively. One issue with verbal fluency tasks is that they confound primary production functions (i.e., the ability to activate, retrieve, and produce a word) with cognitive control functions that are often inherent to these tasks. For example, scoring high on the category fluency task when the target group is “animals” often entails a strategy of focusing on a sub-category (e.g., farm animals), exhausting that, and then successfully switching to a new sub-category (e.g., sea animals; Hirshorn and Thompson-Schill, 2006). This ability requires cognitive control. Without detailed analyses to extract the processes involved in this task, it is unclear whether a correlation between performance and white matter measurements is reflecting a primary production operation or, instead, the implementation of control in language production. This caveat makes the interpretation of findings from these tasks difficult, as will be seen in several of the following sections in which we review the results of correlating performance in verbal fluency tasks with the properties of different tracts. For example, several studies have found a correlation between ILF, IFOF, or both in category fluency tasks (e.g., Almairac et al., 2015; Griffis et al., 2017; Gonzalez et al., 2021), but there are also reports of an association of the same tracts with phonemic fluency performance (e.g., Sanvito et al., 2020). Rather than pointing to the involvement of ILF and IFOF in both semantic and phonological processing, these findings most likely show the underspecified nature of the verbal fluency tasks for pinpointing cognitive functions.

5.4. Summary

In addition to visually guided tasks, including reading, the evidence suggests that ILF and IFOF play a clear role in semantic-lexical processing in language. The evidence seems to point to a more prominent role of the ILF in comprehension and the IFOF in production, although alternative accounts, e.g., a posterior-anterior division of labor in each tract, have not been conclusively ruled out. Finally, there is clear evidence linking IFOF and ILF to semantic-lexical control, although more studies will be useful to examine whether a division along comprehension/production lines also exists in the control functions of these tracts.

6. Uncinate fasciculus (UF)

6.1. Anatomy

The UF (Figure 3) is a short, hook-shaped bidirectional fiber bundle around the Sylvian fissure, running through the extreme and external capsule, and connecting the temporal pole with the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC; Horel and Misantone, 1976; Ebeling and Cramon, 1992; Kier et al., 2004; Von Der Heide et al., 2013; Papinutto et al., 2016; Briggs et al., 2018a). The temporal segment originates from the uncus, entorhinal, and perirhinal cortices, and temporal pole/anterior temporal lobe (Ebeling and Cramon, 1992; Von Der Heide et al., 2013). The frontal termination of the UF has two branches: a larger ventrolateral branch and a smaller medial branch. The ventral branch terminates in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex while the medial branch terminates in the frontal pole (BA 10; Von Der Heide et al., 2013).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Anatomy of UF (A: schematic; B: tractography, sagittal view; C: tractography, coronal view). FP, frontal pole; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; TP, temporal pole.

6.2. Function

Given the essential role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the limbic system in social cognition (e.g., Rushworth et al., 2007) and the extensive connections between the UF and these regions, it is not surprising that abnormalities of the UF have been frequently observed in autism spectrum disorder (ASD; Pugliese et al., 2009; Ameis et al., 2011; Lo et al., 2011; Poustka et al., 2012; Travers et al., 2012), conduct disorder (CD; Passamonti et al., 2012; Sarkar et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2014), social anxiety disorder (Phan et al., 2009; Tröstl et al., 2011; Baur et al., 2012), and schizophrenia (Burns et al., 2003; Kubicki et al., 2005; Kitis et al., 2012; Jung et al., 2020). Also, several studies have demonstrated a correlation between the UF and emotional processing, including the interpretation of emotions and the expression of empathy (Zuurbier et al., 2013; Oishi et al., 2015; Nakajima et al., 2018; Coad et al., 2020; Granger et al., 2021). The connection established between the anterior temporal lobe and orbito-frontal cortex and the bidirectional flow of information in the UF has also led researchers to propose that the tract is heavily involved in modulating mnemonic representations in the temporal lobe through a temporo-frontal reward-punishment loop, i.e., a learning reinforcement loop for forming episodic memory and learning (see Von Der Heide et al., 2013, for a review). Compatible with such a position, the UF has been implicated in learning abilities in both verbal and non-verbal tasks (Thomas et al., 2015; Alm et al., 2016; Rossi et al., 2017), and performance in memory tasks, especially, but not exclusively, verbal memory (Diehl et al., 2008; Fujie et al., 2008; Serra et al., 2012; Christidi et al., 2014; Schaeffer et al., 2014). In terms of lateralization, some studies have reported left-lateralized functions (Kubicki et al., 2002; Hervé et al., 2006; Rodrigo et al., 2007; Hasan et al., 2009), while others have reported right-lateralized functions (Highley et al., 2002; Park et al., 2004), which may be explained, at least in part, by different roles of the tract in cognitive and emotional processing.

6.3. Links to language

This tract’s connection to the ATL also brings up the possibility of involvement in semantic processing. Generally aligned with a role in semantic processing, UF deficits are often observed in dementia, including the semantic variant of PPA (Agosta et al., 2013; Powers et al., 2013; Iaccarino et al., 2015; Tu et al., 2016; Briggs et al., 2018a; Bouchard et al., 2019), with some studies claiming decreased FA of the UF to be the main predictor of semantic dementia (Agosta et al., 2012; Bouchard et al., 2019), and others showing a correlation between cognitive decline and the integrity of the UF (Morikawa et al., 2010; Hiyoshi-Taniguchi et al., 2015). Moreover, the UF has been implicated, along with IFOF, in several studies that have used a battery to tap into semantic processing (de Zubicaray et al., 2011; Mirman et al., 2015; see also semantic processing deficits in schizophrenia; Surbeck et al., 2020). If the UF is involved in semantic selection, one could naturally expect it to be important for a variety of language functions that involve semantic processing. In keeping with this, the UF is also often implicated in language and language disorders. For example, in stroke patients, some studies have reported UF’s FA values to correlate positively with various measures of language processing, including auditory comprehension, naming, and spontaneous speech (Fridriksson et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2021; cf., Ivanova et al., 2016).

In PPA too, UF’s integrity has been linked to naming and category fluency performance (Catani et al., 2013; Powers et al., 2013). Similarly, the microstructure of the UF is predictive of core language scores in children (Dodson et al., 2018). Finally, several studies have implicated the UF in reading abilities, including phonemic decoding efficiency (Bakhtiari et al., 2014; Welcome and Joanisse, 2014; Cummine et al., 2015; Arrington et al., 2017). Evidence directly linking the UF to picture naming is mixed. Some studies have reported that intraoperative stimulation of the UF causes lexical and semantic paraphasia, hesitations, and omission errors in picture naming (Raffa et al., 2016). Others have reported that simple language production tasks like picture naming and counting were not disturbed by the UF stimulation (Duffau et al., 2009). Finally, Jarret et al. (2022) suggested that UF (along with the ILF) constituted an indirect pathway for picture naming.

One possibility is that of a more nuanced picture. Recall that the UF connects ATL to the prefrontal cortex. It is thus possible that its role is not simple semantic retrieval, but semantic control (which is achieved through the interaction between temporal and frontal cortices; e.g., Thompson-Schill et al., 1997). If this is true, one would expect a critical contribution of the UF to tasks with greater control demands on the semantic-lexical system. Specific tests of the role of the UF in semantic-lexical control in tightly controlled studies are rare in the literature, and the current evidence is mixed. Harvey et al. (2013) tested 10 stroke survivors on two tasks that require semantic/semantic-lexical control, Pyramids and Palm Trees (PPTT), and an auditory word-to-picture matching task with semantic, phonological, and unrelated lures. After controlling for total lesion volume, the integrity of UF was a significant predictor of performance both on PPTT and on the word-to-picture matching task with semantic lures, suggesting a role of the UF in semantic/lexical control. Interestingly, this study found no relationship between semantic control and either ILF or IFOF (cf., Harvey and Schnur, 2015). In contrast, Nugiel et al. (2016) found no relationship between performance in the verb generation task, which as explained in the previous section indexes semantic-lexical control when multiple competitors are equally activated, and the microstructural measures of UF, whereas both ILF and IFOF were implicated in that study.

The UF is also frequently linked to performance in semantic fluency tasks in various populations, including individuals with Parkinson and Alzheimer’s disease (Lauro et al., 2010; Papagno et al., 2011, 2016; Rodríguez-Aranda et al., 2016; Di Tella et al., 2020), but as explained in the previous section, it is hard to disentangle semantic retrieval from semantic control in the category fluency tasks. Moreover, several studies have suggested UF’s involvement in phonemic/letter fluency tasks (Papagno et al., 2011; Serra et al., 2012; Kljajevic et al., 2016), even though some have reported greater contribution to semantic over letter fluency performance (Li et al., 2017). If phonemic/letter fluency truly indexes segmental processing abilities, this finding, together with the role of the UF in phonemic decoding proposed in reading, may attribute a dorsal-stream function to a ventral tract, a conclusion that does not fit well with the rest of this tract’s function, or with the null effects regarding its influence on segmental processing (e.g., no phonological errors or disruption in phonological processing during its stimulation; Duffau et al., 2009; Nomura et al., 2013; Raffa et al., 2016). A more plausible explanation is that the phonemic/letter fluency tasks also tap into other abilities, e.g., controlled lexical selection, that are more aligned with the functions of the UF. This can be tested by ruling out UF’s function in tasks that require phoneme/letter processing without strong demands on semantic-lexical selection.

One last finding that is relevant here is the selective impairment of naming pictures of famous people and objects when the UF is removed: in a study of 44 glioma patients before and after the surgical removal of UF, Papagno et al. (2011) reported changes to word production in picture naming, naming of famous faces, as well as impaired performance on verbal fluency tasks. However, a later study that followed up on 17 glioma patients up to 9 months after their surgery, showed that performance on picture naming and verbal fluency tasks had been restored to normal, but patients still had a significant impairment in the famous-face naming task (Papagno et al., 2016). It is possible that this selective impairment is linked to emotional processing (Papagno et al., 2016) or to semantic control and selection, both of which are more marked for famous faces and places than for generic objects.

6.4. Summary

UF’s connections to the orbito-frontal cortex imply a clear role for this region in processing reward and punishment, which is connected to both social/emotional processing and reward-based learning. These functions can affect various aspects of language processing, especially those with a social component. The question is whether, beyond these, the linking of ATL to the prefrontal cortex implies a role for the UF in semantic-lexical selection in situations with high selection demands. Current results sometimes implicate the UF, sometimes ILF and/or IFOF, and sometimes a combination of these tracts. One interpretation of these diverging results is that the UF plays a complementary role to ILF and IFOF in semantic processing (Cocquyt et al., 2020). More empirical data will be helpful in testing this hypothesis.

7. Extreme capsule (EmC)

7.1. Anatomy

The EmC (Figure 4) is often mentioned in studies of white matter. However, researchers vary greatly in their definition of what the EmC is. Some view it simply as a topographical region between the insula and claustrum (Axer et al., 2013). Others have described it as part of the IFOF or UF or the MdLF/ILF pathway (Saur et al., 2008; Northam et al., 2012; Patterson et al., 2014; Verly et al., 2019). Yet others have described it as a more substantial fiber complex or system, e.g., the “extreme capsule fiber complex” (Mars et al., 2016) or the “extreme capsule fascicle” (Martinez Oeckel et al., 2021), and have included large sections of the ventral pathway in this bundle. Finally, in a careful study focused specifically on delineating the trajectory of EmC, Makris and Pandya (2008) were able to clearly distinguish between the tract and the adjacent MdLF, UF, AF, and SLF-II, and SLF-III. They defined EmC as a long, left-lateralized association fiber coursing between the inferior frontal cortex and the STG, extending into the inferior parietal lobule.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Anatomy of EmC. EmC, extreme capsule; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; IPL, inferior parietal lobe; STG, superior temporal gyrus. The fibers of this tract overlap with IFOF, and UF, and therefore tractography of this tract was not possible.

7.2. Function and links to language

Given the temporal origin of the tract which is close to Heschel’s gyrus, auditory processing has been one of the proposed functions of this tract (Frey et al., 2008). More nuanced hypotheses have been formed around the connection that this tract provides between the STG and IFG, in terms of mediating verbal working memory. For example, Lopez-Barroso et al. (2011) showed that performance under articulatory suppression (asking participants to repeatedly produce the syllable “bla” while listening to the language stream) was significantly correlated with FA in left EmC and the external capsule. Naturally, a role in verbal working memory links EmC to language learning. In line with this hypothesis, Wong et al. (2011) trained participants in a sound-to-word learning paradigm, where they had to use foreign phonetic contrasts to access meaning. They found the FA of a left temporo-parietal region to be correlated with learning in this paradigm, and reported EmC, along with ILF, to mediate auditory comprehension.

Studying 25 individuals with aphasia and 24 healthy controls, Kourtidou et al. (2021) found that radial diffusivity of the right temporo-frontal extreme capsule fasciculus was predictive of a number of language functions, including oral and reading comprehension, word and sentence repetition, and number of words/minute produced in storytelling tasks, such as the Cookie Theft Picture Test (see also Martinez Oeckel et al., 2021). Others have proposed EmC’s involvement in various language functions including speech rate in stroke survivors (Efthymiopoulou et al., 2017), semantic paraphasia in glioma patients in intra-operative stimulation (Duffau et al., 2005), comprehension (Kümmerer et al., 2013), picture naming (Jarret et al., 2022), and vocabulary development in children with developmental language disorders (Verly et al., 2019), while others have reported no relationship between EmC’s lesion size and language production abilities such as naming and rate and informativeness of speech (e.g., Marchina et al., 2011). Finally, impaired syntactic processing has also been linked to EmC deficits in some studies (e.g., Papoutsi et al., 2011; Rolheiser et al., 2011; Griffiths et al., 2012), but not in others (Wilson et al., 2011; Teichmann et al., 2015).

7.3. Summary

To summarize, EmC’s function is not well understood, partly due to the lack of consensus about its anatomical definition, but there is more and more evidence that the tract represents a pathway distinct from its neighboring white matter, and may be involved in some aspect of language processing. So far, detailed studies of the linguistic function of the EmC have been relatively sparse, but the connection between STG and frontal cortex, and its implication for sound processing and verbal working memory, appears to be a promising venue for more theoretical approaches to the possible function of this tract in language acquisition, comprehension, and production.

8. Middle longitudinal fasciculus (MdLF)

8.1. Anatomy

The MdLF (Figure 5) is a long association fiber that connects temporal regions with parietal and occipital lobes (Burks et al., 2017; Conner et al., 2018b). First reported by Seltzer and Pandya (1984) using autoradiographic histological tract-tracing and later confirmed using more recent non-human tract-tracing studies (Schmahmann et al., 2007), the pathway had been historically absent from human anatomical reports (Burdach, 1826; Foville, 1844; Meynert, 1885; Dejerine, 1895) and even some recent anatomical atlases (e.g., Oishi et al., 2010) and studies of white matter tracts (Catani et al., 2005; Bürgel et al., 2006; Wakana et al., 2007; Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten, 2008; Hua et al., 2008; Holl et al., 2011; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2011). More recent studies in humans, however, have begun to identify MdLF as a distinct pathway. Some, such as Saur et al. (2008) discuss the MdLF as two composite fiber bundles, one in the dorsal pathway together with AF/SLF and one in the ventral pathway together with the ILF. Others have identified the MdLF as an independent tract extending from the AG to the anterior superior temporal cortex, running dorsal and medial to the AF/SLF (Frey et al., 2008; Makris and Pandya, 2008; Turken and Dronkers, 2011; Wong et al., 2011; Menjot de Champfleur et al., 2013). Today, researchers agree that the tract is heavily involved in connecting STG to other parts of the cortex, although there is not always consensus among studies on what these other parts are. Candidates include other regions in the temporal cortex, such as MTG (Turken and Dronkers, 2011), parietal regions such as the superior parietal lobule/precuneus and AG (Turken and Dronkers, 2011; Wang et al., 2013; Makris et al., 2013), and possibly some of the occipital regions such as the cuneus, and lateral occipital lobe (Makris et al., 2017). In one of the most recent attempts to define the anatomical branches of the MdLF, Kalyvas et al. (2020) performed a combined study of cadaveric dissections together with DTI in neurotypical adult participants and identified three branches of the MdLF (Figure 5): the first, MdLF-I, connects TP and STG to the SPL through Heschel’s gyrus. The second, MdLF-II, connects TP and STG to the parieto-occipital regions. The third, MdLF-III connects the most anterior part of TP to the posterior part of the occipital lobe through the AG.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Anatomy of MdLF (A: schematic; B: tractography, sagittal view; C: tractography, coronal view). OL, occipital lobe; POR, parieto-occipital regions; TP, temporal pole.

8.2. Function and links to language

Based on its clear connections to the temporal lobe and AG, there have been attempts to link MdLF to language processing. For example, by bundling the AF/SLF with one branch of the MdLF and ILF with another branch of MdLF, Saur et al. (2008) proposed the former’s role in sublexical repetition and the latter’s role in comprehension in 33 neurotypical adults. However, because of bundling with other fibers in the dorsal and lateral pathways, respectively, it is difficult to isolate the role of MdLF in such tasks. MdLF has not been a frequent target of aphasia studies, but the little evidence that exists is mixed. In a study of 20 individuals with PPA, Luo et al. (2020) reported significant correlations between word comprehension and naming and the white matter changes to the MdLF in the dominant hemisphere. In contrast, Blom-Smink et al. (2020) found no clear link between the integrity of MdLF and naming performance in 10 individuals with sub-acute post-stroke aphasia. Finally, in one of the few intraoperative electrostimulation studies investigating the role of MdLF in language, De Witt Hamer et al. (2011) tested counting and picture naming of eight glioma patients after the stimulation of MdLF and found no changes to either task. Moreover, the resection of the left MdLF did not result in impaired naming. It must be noted, however, that resection was not complete in all patients, and only included the part of the MdLF anterior to Heschel’s gyrus, therefore, these results must be interpreted with caution.

The brief review above highlights the sparsity of research on the role of MdLF in language processing, but the few existing results do not seem to provide strong support for an essential role of this tract at least in language production, including the semantic-lexical mapping required for picture naming. Two findings in the anatomical study of Kalyvas et al. (2020) provide theoretical support for the non-critical role of MdLF in language processing: (1) there is no clear termination of the fibers from any of the MdLF branches in the AG, and (2) No clear leftward symmetry of the tract (also see Makris and Pandya, 2008; Makris et al., 2013, 2017; Kamali et al., 2014 for differences in the asymmetry index). In contrast, the centrality of STG and the auditory cortex in at least two branches of this tract motivates a role in auditory processing (Saur et al., 2010; Dick and Tremblay, 2012). Evidence in support of this view is more convincing, although there is much room for additional evidence and careful studies. For example, MdLF was one of the ventral pathways implicated in the study of Wong et al. (2011) where they measured participants’ ability to learn new phonetic contrasts for discriminating words in a foreign language. But perhaps the most detailed study of the role of MdLF in auditory processing is the study of Tremblay et al. (2019), in which the authors used high angular resolution diffusion imaging (HARDI) with advanced anatomically constrained particle filtering tractography algorithms that are robust against problems such as crossing fibers and partial volume effects, to disentangle the role of AF and MdLF in auditory processing. Younger and older adults participated in a syllable discrimination task with broadband masking noise. After controlling for differences in individuals’ hearing sensitivity, an age-independent effect linked both tracts to performance in the task, but in relatively distinct ways: while AF was predictive of sensitivity (d-prime in the signal detection framework), the MdLF was linked to response bias (criterion in the signal detection framework). These results suggest a distinct role for MdLF in higher-level auditory processing, such as decision making.

8.3. Summary

The MdLF is a relatively understudied tract. But recent evidence suggests that it is a distinct tract in humans and that it has possibly up to three separate branches. Of the roles proposed so far for this tract, an involvement in auditory processing is the most plausible and well-supported role. The nature of such involvement, however, remains underspecified. Future studies should clarify the extent to which the processing is speech-specific (or not), and whether the tract’s role is more pronounced in cognitive aspects of auditory processing, such as implicit or explicit decision making. Finally, the links to the occipital cortex remain intriguing, and potentially related to processes mediating audiovisual integration (Wang et al., 2013), although an empirical investigation of this hypothesis has, to our knowledge, not yet been carried out.

9. Superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF)

9.1. Anatomy

When we think about the classic language pathway, we often think about Geschwind’s iconic illustration of a pathway connecting Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas. For years, variations of this pathway connecting the inferior frontal cortex with temporal and parietal lobes comprised a non-dissociable SLF/AF bundle, which has been called by various names, including the Burdach fasciculus, the superior longitudinal bundle or the arcuate bundle (e.g., Burdach, 1826; Dejerine, 1895; Wernicke et al., 1897). More recently, the two tracts have been deemed distinct, although AF is still widely considered as one of the branches of the SLF.

SLF (Figure 6) is a bundle of association fibers that connects the superior and inferior parietal cortices to the frontal cortex (Petrides and Pandya, 1984; Yeterian et al., 2012). The SLF is usually divided into three distinct branches, SLF-I, SLF-II, and SLF-III (Petrides and Pandya, 1984, 2002, Petrides and Pandya, 2006; Yeterian et al., 2012; Caverzasi et al., 2016; Barbeau et al., 2020), although some disagreement remains about the exact origin and destination of each branch. SLF-I is the dorsal-most branch, and connects the superior parietal lobule and precuneus to the superior frontal cortex, the dorsal premotor area, the SMA, and possibly the anterior cingulate cortex (Petrides and Pandya, 1984; Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006; Schmahmann et al., 2007; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2012; Yeterian et al., 2012). SLF-II originates in the caudal inferior parietal lobule, the intraparietal sulcus, and the angular gyrus and terminates in the DLPFC, including the dorsal premotor area (Petrides and Pandya, 1984, 2002, Petrides and Pandya, 2006; Yeterian et al., 2012). The SLF-III is the ventral-most branch and connects the rostral part of the inferior parietal lobule, i.e., the supramarginal gyrus, and the anterior parts of the intraparietal sulcus to the ventral premotor cortex and the caudal banks of the arcuate and principal sulci (Petrides and Pandya, 1984, 2002, Petrides and Pandya, 2006; Yeterian et al., 2012). Recently, Barbeau et al. (2020) proposed a division of SLF-III into two branches, the ventral branch terminating in BA 6 (pre-SMA and SMA) and BA 44 (pars opercularis of IFG), and the dorsal branch terminating in BA 9 and BA 46 (DLPFC). Finally, some studies describe a temporoparietal component of the SLF, which traverses from the posterior part of the STG to the inferior and superior parietal lobules, and is often labeled SLF-tp (e.g., Caverzasi et al., 2016), and is sometimes further divided into SLF-tp-IPL (inferior parietal lobule) and tp-SPL (superior parietal lobule; Kamali et al., 2014; Bullock et al., 2019). It is worth mentioning that AF, which courses in parallel to SLF-III and connects the temporoparietal junction to the frontal cortex, has sometimes been considered a part of the larger SLF bundle. In this article, we will discuss AF separately in the next section.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Anatomy of SLF (A: schematic; B: tractography, axial view of the SLF II; C: tractography, sagittal view of the SLF II; D: tractography, axial view of the SLF III; E: tractography, sagittal view of the SLF III; SLF-I is not shown due to lack of relevance to language processing). DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; dPMC, dorsal premotor cortex; IPS, intraparietal sulcus; Poper, Pars opercularis; SPL, superior parietal lobule; TP, temporoparietal.

9.2. Function

Empirical studies do not always separate the three branches of the SLF, but in those which do, it is often the SLF-III and sometimes the SLF-II that are related to language processing. Due to its anatomical location and connections, SLF-I is usually not considered a language-related tract. Its main connectivity is to the superior parietal cortex, which encodes body part locations in relation to space and eye-hand coordination for reaching. Consequently, damage to the caudal part of the superior parietal lobule can cause optic ataxia, i.e., difficulty in visually guided reach (Naito et al., 2008; Ferraina et al., 2009; Granek et al., 2012). By connecting this region to the premotor cortex, SLF-I is thought to play a role in regulating fine motor behavior, especially in tasks that require selection among competing motor plans (Hyde et al., 2021). SLF-tp is not always mentioned, but together with AF, there are reports of its connection to the language (Caverzasi et al., 2016). Galantucci et al. (2011) claimed to have separated SLF-tp from the temporoparietal AF and showed that only damage to the former was observed in the logopenic variant of PPA. They also reported SLF-tp’s damage in the nonfluent variant of PPA.

9.3. Links to language

The investigation of the linguistic functions of the SLF has ranged from broad to specific aspects of language processing. For example, in a study of 20 children between 8 and 10 years of age, Asaridou et al. (2017) showed that children’s vocabulary growth was uniquely predicted by the cortical thickness of the left SMG, and concluded that the direct link between this region and the IFG provided by SLF-III makes the tract a critical pathway for the development of the lexicon. Likewise, in a comparison between children on the autism spectrum with and without language impairment, Nagae et al. (2012) linked elevated mean diffusivity values of SLF to language impairments. More recently, Gao et al. (2020) showed increased FA in the right SLF in bilingual compared to monolingual speakers (but see Wang et al., 2016, for reports of left-lateralization of SLF-II and SLF-III). The link is stronger for production than comprehension. For example, in a study of 49 typically developing children and adolescents ranging from 5–17 years of age, Urger et al. (2015) reported that only production but not comprehension was associated with the FA of left SLF. Likewise, Hillis et al. (2018) reported a link between naming recovery in post-stroke aphasia and the integrity of SLF/AF.

More detailed hypotheses usually concern the role of SLF-III: by the virtue of connecting the SMG, which is frequently implicated in phonological processing and phonological deficits (e.g., Schwartz et al., 2012), to the frontal cortex, this branch of SLF is a prime candidate for language processing, especially phonological production. The more recent division into ventral and dorsal branches terminating in premotor and DLPFC, respectively (Barbeau et al., 2020), has further motivated finer-grained distinctions in the function of this tract into mapping phonological codes onto abstract motor plans (Bohland et al., 2010; Miller and Guenther, 2021) vs. maintaining phonological information in working memory. In keeping with this hypothesized role, several lines of research have linked phonological processing abilities to SLF (and in some cases, specifically to SLF-III). First, measures of phonological awareness are often correlated with the properties of SLF. The tests used to measure phonological awareness vary, but often include tasks such as sound matching, elision, and word blending. In sound matching tasks, participants hear two or more stimuli and must make a perceptual judgment about whether they match or not. Elision tasks measure the ability to remove phonemes from spoken words to form other words, and blending words tasks measure the ability to synthesize phonemes to form words. In a comparison of two groups of children born pre-term and full-term, tested when 6 years old, Dodson et al. (2018) showed a significant association between the FA of the left SLF (and AF) and phonological awareness measures (see also Travis et al., 2017).

A second phenomenon linking SLF to phonological abilities in production is the Tip of the Tongue (TOT) state. This is a state in which the speaker almost remembers a word but cannot fully produce it. TOT states usually benefit from phonological cues more than semantic cues, which has led to the localization of the problem to the mapping of lexical items to phonemes (Meyer and Bock, 1992). Stamatakis et al. (2011) tested 24 neurotypical adults between 19 and 82 years of age on the Boston Naming Task and showed that even though accuracy was associated with several tracts, including SLF, the TOT state was uniquely related to the most posterior part of the left SLF and a homologous area in the right hemisphere (cf., Kljajevic and Erramuzpe, 2019). A third line of research linking SLF to phonological abilities is the neuropsychological data on word production. McKinnon et al. (2018) tested 32 participants with chronic post-stroke aphasia and showed that the probability of making semantic and phonological errors was strongly linked to the ILF and SLF axonal loss, respectively (see also Han et al., 2016; Kyeong et al., 2019; Stark et al., 2019). Similarly, in a study of 24 participants with PPA, Powers et al. (2013) showed that participants’ naming performance in the logopenic—but not the semantic—variant was associated with the integrity of the left SLF. Moreover, auditory repetition performance, which has a stronger emphasis on phonological encoding than lexical retrieval (Nozari et al., 2010; Dell et al., 2013; Nozari and Dell, 2013), has also been linked to the SLF (Breier et al., 2008). Adding to this evidence are data from individuals who stutter. Chang et al. (2015) studied 77 children who stuttered and reported that these children, compared to controls, showed decreased FA of the left SLF and the gray matter regions connected by this tract, including IFG, premotor, motor, STG, MTG, and inferior parietal regions. Likewise, intraoperative stimulation of SLF has led to dysarthria and anarthria (Maldonado et al., 2011), although distinguishing problems of articulation from phonological encoding is difficult, especially during surgery with limited time.

Finally, the last line of research connecting the SLF to phonological processing is the close link posited between the tract’s properties and reading abilities (e.g., Bakhtiari et al., 2014; Travis et al., 2017; Bruckert et al., 2019). SLF is one of the three major pathways related to the VWFA, providing critical connections between this region, STG, IFG, and DLPFC (Chen et al., 2019). Bruckert et al. (2019) showed that the FA values for the left and right SLF and left AF at 6 years of age were predictive of the oral reading index at age 8. These findings are mirrored in studies of dyslexia: Zhao et al. (2016) showed that, as a group, children with dyslexia demonstrated greater right-lateralization of SLF-II (together with less left-lateralization of IFOF) than non-dyslexic readers. The lateralization index was also predictive of reading performance in the dyslexic group (see also Hoeft et al., 2011). Even though reading requires more than just phonological processing abilities, such abilities do play an important role, especially in sublexical reading. Therefore, although not a sufficient piece of evidence by itself, the link between SLF and reading abilities adds to the other bodies of evidence connecting this tract to phonological processing abilities.

There are also many studies investigating the correlation between SLF properties and verbal fluency tasks. As noted in earlier sections, although verbal fluency is one of the most popular language tests in studies of the neurobiology of language, it is not nearly as pure of a measure for semantic and phonological abilities as it has often been assumed. Unsurprisingly, even though SLF has been implicated in both semantic and phonemic fluency tasks, these reports are not consistent across studies and populations (e.g., Powers et al., 2013; Madhavan et al., 2014; Pustina et al., 2014; Urger et al., 2015; Sanvito et al., 2020; Gonzalez et al., 2021), and in most cases do not differentially implicate SLF’s involvement in one task vs. the other. Rather than taking such evidence as suggesting a role for SLF in both semantic and phonological processing, it is plausible to consider the tract’s relevance to some general task demands shared by both phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks. For example, it has been suggested that the association between SLF and verbal fluency in studies of aging is in fact mediated by verbal working memory, which has been linked to the FA of the bilateral SLF (Peters et al., 2012).

The studies reviewed above clearly link SLF to phonological processing. More sporadic have been attempts to link SLF to syntactic processing. In a study of 17 neurotypical children, Mills et al. (2013) found that those with a more complex narrative had higher diffusion coefficients in their left SLF and (AF; see also Friederici et al., 2006 for implicating SLF in parsing sentences with complex hierarchical structures). A few neuropsychological studies have also provided support for the link between SLF and syntactic processing. Wilson et al. (2011) studied 27 participants with PPA and found reduced FA in the left SLF/AF to be correlated with syntactic deficits in both comprehension and production. In another PPA study, Marcotte et al. (2017) linked the deficits in the non-fluent variant (nfv) vs. the semantic variant (sv) to increased radial diffusivity in the left SLF vs. in the bilateral UF and ILF, respectively (see also Whitwell et al., 2010; Tetzloff et al., 2018). There is also a report of inducing grammatical gender errors produced in response to noun probe in nine French-speaking glioma patients undergoing surgery, as a result of what appears to be the stimulation of a part of SLF (Vidorreta et al., 2011).

Although not as clear as the motivation for linking SLF to phonological processing, it is possible that parts of the tract traversing from the parietal to the frontal cortex may connect the pMTG and IFG and thus carry out the syntactic operations proposed by Matchin and Hickok (2020) in production. More difficult is explaining the role of the tract in syntactic comprehension unless one notes the generally high demands on working memory and control processes often required in sentences with complex hierarchical structures that have often been used in experiments tapping into syntactic comprehension. In such cases, it is conceivable that the DLPFC and IFG’s connection to the temporoparietal regions may be essential for maintaining information actively in working memory and resolving competition between different interpretations. This is particularly important, as SLF, and especially SLF-II and SLF-III, have been independently implicated in working memory (Peters et al., 2012; Rizio and Diaz, 2016) as well as executive control (Urger et al., 2015; Ramsey et al., 2017; Linortner et al., 2020). Finally, unless carefully controlled in the experimental materials, syntactic complexity is often confounded with length, which itself imposes higher demands on phonological working memory, providing yet another alternative interpretation for the role of SLF in syntactic processing.

9.4. Summary

A rather large and converging body of evidence points to a clear role of the SLF (especially SLF-III, and possibly SLF-II) in phonological processing, especially in language production. SLF-tp is a promising but understudied candidate. Further studies can focus on disentangling the functions of the components of SLF, such as the maintenance of phonological information in working memory vs. mapping such information onto motor plans. There is also some evidence for the involvement of this tract in syntactic processing, but potential confounds such as higher demands on verbal working memory and executive control processes must be ruled out before the tract can be claimed to have a pure “syntactic” function. This is another great avenue for future research.

10. Arcuate fasciculus (AF)

10.1. Anatomy

The arcuate fasciculus (Figure 7) is a dorsal tract that connects the posterior superior temporal cortex (pSTC) to the IFG and ventral premotor cortex (vPMC; Catani et al., 2005, 2012; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2011; Weiner et al., 2017; Tremblay et al., 2019). Its anatomy is still under debate, and some studies suggested that the posterior part of the AF runs into the MTG and ITG (Rilling et al., 2008; Bernard et al., 2019; Giampiccolo and Duffau, 2022). Nevertheless, there is agreement that the tract has multiple branches (Dick and Tremblay, 2012; Dick et al., 2014). One common classification divides the tract into direct and indirect segments (Catani et al., 2005). The direct segment connects pSTC to IFG, MFG, and vPMC, while the indirect segment itself further divides into two components, a posterior component connecting STC to the inferior parts of the parietal lobe, and an anterior component connecting the inferior parietal areas to IFG, MFG, and vPMC (Catani et al., 2005, 2012; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2011; Weiner et al., 2017; Tremblay et al., 2019). A different model, the dual pathway architecture (Berwick et al., 2013; Brauer et al., 2013), also exists and is of potential functional importance. The key difference between the two branches in this model is not their origin (which is presumed to be pSTC in both cases), but rather their termination points, i.e., vPMC vs. IFG. It is hypothesized that the branch connecting pSTC and vPMC is involved in sensory-motor mapping and phonological processing, whereas the branch connecting pSTC to IFG is involved in higher-level processing, possibly syntactic processing. A critical difference between the AF in non-human primates and humans is that the tract is not strongly lateralized in monkeys (Eichert et al., 2019), but the evidence points to a prominent left-lateralization in humans (Vernooij et al., 2007; Lebel and Beaulieu, 2009; Sreedharan et al., 2015; Allendorfer et al., 2016; Silva and Citterio, 2017; Travis et al., 2017; Bruckert et al., 2019; Eichert et al., 2019; cf., Yeatman et al., 2011).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Anatomy of AF (A: schematic; B: tractography, coronal view; C: tractography, sagittal view). AG, angular gyrus; AsAF, anterior segment of the arcuate fasciculus; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; LsAF, long segment of the arcuate fasciculus; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; PsAF, Posterior segment of the arcuate fasciculus; STS, superior temporal sulcus; vPMC, ventral Premotor Cortex.

10.2. Function and links to language

The left-lateralization of AF in humans suggests a role of this tract in language processing. In line with this, the empirical evidence has often linked the left AF to language processing. For example, in a study of 106 neurotypical adults, Teubner-Rhodes et al. (2016) reported a positive correlation between vocabulary knowledge and microstructural properties of the left AF in the segment of the tract behind the posterior part of the Sylvian fissure. Similarly, the AF lesions have been linked to language deficits in aphasia (e.g., Tak and Jang, 2014; cf., Forkel et al., 2020), as well as language recovery (Primaßin et al., 2015). The critical role of the AF, irrespective of the damage to the gray matter, has been recently demonstrated in a study of 134 stroke survivors whose lesions were relatively limited to the left frontal cortex. Gajardo-Vidal et al. (2021) showed that production impairment lasting beyond 3 months after the stroke was selectively related to the damage of the anterior part of the AF directly above the insula, regardless of the damage to Broca’s area (see also Hillis et al., 2018). Interestingly, production scores were lower in the case of the AF damage with an intact Broca’s area compared to Broca’s damage with an intact AF, ruling out a central role for Broca’s damage in the impairment.

Some have claimed the involvement of the AF in both language production and comprehension (Turken and Dronkers, 2011). For example, Broce et al. (2015) showed that microstructural properties of this tract were predictive of both expressive and receptive language in children of 5–8 years of age. More recent studies have begun to localize these functions to different segments of the AF. For example, in a study of 33 individuals with aphasia, Ivanova et al. (2021) showed that AF’s anterior segment was predictive of fluency and naming, and its posterior segment of comprehension abilities. On the perception side, the role of the AF has been most extensively studied in auditory processing. Several studies have compared musicians and non-musicians in their ability to process fine-grained details of sound and have linked the superior performance in the former to different properties of the AF in that group (Oechslin et al., 2010; Moore et al., 2017; Li et al., 2021; cf. Perron et al., 2021). Interestingly, the volume and microstructural complexity of the tract also differ between vocal and instrumental musicians, compatible with the idea that the tract plays a special role in vocal-motor processing (Halwani et al., 2011). In non-musicians too, basic auditory processes have been linked to the microstructural properties of the AF (e.g., Vaquero et al., 2021). In a more detailed study, Tremblay et al. (2019) showed that sensitivity in a syllable discrimination task was related to the properties of the AF, while response bias was related to MdLF, putting AF in the center of the operations involved in fine-grained speech sound processing.

But the bulk of evidence, so far, is in favor of a role in production. In a study of 30 post-stroke individuals in the chronic phase, Marchina et al. (2011) showed that lesions to AF uniquely predicted naming ability, as well as the rate and informativeness of speech. In a study of 31 post-stroke individuals in the chronic phase, Halai et al. (2017) showed that speech fluency was uniquely correlated with the anterior section of the arcuate fasciculus. Several other studies in individuals with brain damage have also linked AF lesions to the fluency of spoken production (Fridriksson et al., 2013; Basilakos et al., 2014; Chenausky et al., 2020; Ivanova et al., 2021). Note that by “fluency” here, we mean fluency in the context of connected speech, as opposed to tests of “verbal fluency.” As alluded to in several of the earlier sections, the results of those tests are difficult to interpret. The issue is also evident in the AF literature: while some studies have reported a correlation between verbal fluency scores and the properties of AF (e.g., Blecher et al., 2019; Sanvito et al., 2020; Gonzalez et al., 2021), others have not (Phillips et al., 2011; Costentin et al., 2019). Moreover, the reports of AF lateralization are also not consistent across studies. Some have reported the involvement of the left (Blecher et al., 2019; Sanvito et al., 2020) and others of the right (Gonzalez et al., 2021) AF in letter fluency. Similarly, some studies have implicated the left (Blecher et al., 2019) and others of the right (Gonzalez et al., 2021) AF in semantic fluency. As for fluency in connected speech, there are many contributing factors (Nozari and Faroqi-Shah, 2017).

More evidence for the role of the AF in lower-level processing in production comes from its involvement in auditory repetition (Breier et al., 2008; Kim and Jang, 2013; Shinoura et al., 2013), especially sublexical repetition which indexes mapping input phonology to output phonology (Saur et al., 2008; Sierpowska et al., 2017). Similarly, damage to the AF has been shown to be specifically associated with phonological errors (Schwartz et al., 2012) as opposed to semantic errors, which arise at higher levels of processing in the production system (Nozari et al., 2011; Dell et al., 2014). This finding has been corroborated by the intraoperative stimulation of the AF or the AF termination sites, which has often led to speech errors (Giampiccolo et al., 2020), anomia (Duffau et al., 2002), and most pointedly, phonological errors (Mandonnet et al., 2007). Disentangling phonemic errors from dysarthric errors is not easy and has not been a focus in many of the studies reviewed above. It is thus possible that some of the problems reported in such studies are due to articulatory issues. In line with this idea, Liégeois et al. (2013) compared 32 individuals with a history of childhood TBI, and showed that those with persistent dysarthria had reduced FA in the left AF and reduced volume of the left AF and corpus callosum compared to those without dysarthria.

The critical role of the AF in phonological processing has also been tested with an array of phonological awareness tests. Different studies use different measures, and sometimes a mix of perception and production, but common tests include sound matching, elision, and word blending (see task definitions under the SLF section). The reports link the AF (often along with SLF, and often on the left side) to phonological awareness (Yeatman et al., 2011; Saygin et al., 2013; Gullick and Booth, 2014, 2015; Vandermosten et al., 2015; Dodson et al., 2018). Some researchers have also suggested that the auditory-motor mapping role of AF entails storing phonological information in working memory, essential for learning complex phonological sequences (Schulze et al., 2012). In keeping with this idea, López-Barroso et al. (2013) demonstrated a negative correlation between word learning and radial diffusivity of the long segment of the left AF in a group of adult participants learning an artificial language. Similar evidence followed in a longitudinal study of vocabulary development in children; Su et al. (2018b) followed the developmental trajectory of 79 children from age 4–14 years and reported a correlation between vocabulary development in the direct and posterior segments of the AF in the left hemisphere.

The AF has also been strongly linked to reading, at least in part due to its role in phonological processing. Thiebaut de Schotten et al. (2014) showed that increased FA and decreased perpendicular diffusivity of the temporoparietal portion of the left AF accompanied literacy. Furthermore, the microstructure of the AF was correlated with the response of VWFA to letter strings. In a longitudinal study of 30 children between the ages of 8 and 14 years, Gullick and Booth (2014) found a correlation between reading development and the FA of the direct segment of the AF for both younger and older halves of their sample (see also Yeatman et al., 2012; Gullick and Booth, 2015). The correlation between the AF and reading abilities has also been reported in children with dyslexia. In a longitudinal study of 75 children from ages 5–6–7–8 years, Vanderauwera et al. (2017) showed that only the left AF was exclusively related to the development of dyslexia (see also Hoeft et al., 2011). Also, a comparison of children with a family risk of dyslexia with a control sample showed that at-risk children had lower FA in the posterior AF (as well as the left IFOF; Vandermosten et al., 2015).

A special connection has also been suggested between the AF, as the tract connecting auditory to motor regions, and conduction aphasia, a deficit of mapping sensory to motor speech (Benson et al., 1973). For instance, individuals with conduction aphasia often show good semantic processing, but make phonological errors in naming. Importantly, their auditory repetition performance is markedly impaired compared to their naming performance. One suggestion has been that conduction aphasia may result from damage to the AF. There has been some empirical support for this idea, with lesions severely damaging AF and the surrounding tissue causing phonemic paraphasia and notable repetition deficits (e.g., Tanabe et al., 1987; Yamada et al., 2007). However, lesions to the AF do not necessarily cause conduction aphasia (e.g., Shuren et al., 1995; Selnes et al., 2002; Epstein-Peterson et al., 2012), suggesting that AF damage is not sufficient to produce this disorder.

Finally, some have attributed higher-level processing functions, such as syntactic processing, to AF. In one study, Mills et al. (2013) found a trend linking the syntactic complexity of the sentences produced by children to the diffusivity measures of the left AF. In another study, Papoutsi et al. (2011) found the AF lesions to be predictive of syntactic processing impairment. But it is important to note, as alluded to in the SLF section, that increasing syntactic complexity is often accompanied by increasing phonological complexity, i.e., longer phrases, more embeddings, etc. Therefore, unless specifically controlled for phonological processing load, drawing conclusions about the AF’s direct involvement in syntactic processing would be difficult.

10.3. Summary

The evidence reviewed above shows a clear role for AF in processing auditory details, mapping sound to motor actions, and more generally, phonological processing, especially in production. A possible role has also been proposed for syntactic processing, but so far, the evidence supporting this proposal has been limited. Moreover, careful controls for other factors that may increase processing load have not always been implemented in studies assessing the syntactic functions of AF. Disentangling phonological load from syntactic load would be a fruitful avenue for future studies on AF. Another fruitful avenue for future research would be to separate phonological and articulatory phonetic functions of AF (and SLF).

11. Frontal aslant tract (FAT)

11.1. Anatomy

The FAT (Figure 8) is a short association fiber connecting the lateral IFG to the SMA and pre-SMA in the superior frontal gyrus (Catani et al., 2012, 2013). Although a tract with these characteristics had been discussed before (e.g., Aron et al., 2007; Lawes et al., 2008; Oishi et al., 2008), the term “Aslant tract” was first coined by Catani et al. (2012); see also Thiebaut de Schotten et al. (2012), who described it specifically as a pathway connecting IFG’s pars opercularis to pre-SMA (see also Bozkurt et al., 2016). Today, the existence of this tract and its role in connecting IFG and the superior frontal gyrus is well established in both primates and humans (Petrides and Pandya, 2002; Martino and De Lucas, 2014; Briggs et al., 2018b).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Anatomy of FAT (A: schematic; B: tractography, coronal view; C: tractography, sagittal view). IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; SMA, supplementary motor area.

11.2. Function and links to language

Based on its connectivity, several functions have been proposed for the role of FAT in cognition. Due to its clear connection to IFG, the debates that have impacted the role of IFG have directly impacted the ideas about the role of FAT. On the one hand, the classic view of Broca’s area as a key language region has generated the hypothesis that FAT’s connection to this region makes it a key language tract (e.g., Tremblay and Dick, 2016). The tract’s connection to pre-SMA and SMA and the link between language production and these regions has further supported FAT’s role in language processing (Tremblay and Gracco, 2009; Jarret et al., 2022). The alternative, domain-general view of IFG (e.g., Nozari et al., 2016a; cf. Nozari and Novick, 2017), on the other hand, has led some researchers to implicate FAT in conflict resolution functions, in both linguistic and non-linguistic domains, that have been attributed to this region (Dick et al., 2019). This view is further reinforced by the fact that pre-SMA and SMA have often been implicated in complex motor tasks, especially those that involve high competition (Derrfuss et al., 2004; Mars et al., 2007), such as the Flanker task (Ullsperger and von Cramon, 2001). Consequently, FAT’s links to these regions have been taken as a possible involvement of the tract in motor selection, conflict monitoring and resolution, and the execution of motor plans regardless of specific domains (e.g., Tremblay and Small, 2011). Dick et al. (2019) present a thorough review of the linguistic and non-linguistic functions of FAT, with an eye towards the tract’s laterality. Below, we briefly review the evidence and the conclusions.

The evidence linking the FAT to language processing and language disorders is ample. For example, a large-scale study of 834 participants from the Human Connectome Project found a significant correlation between bilateral FAT volume and language performance (Varriano et al., 2018). In another study, Broce et al. (2015) found that the length of the left FAT was predictive of language comprehension abilities in young children. Moreover, in stroke survivors, the extent of damage to FAT is predictive of the improvement of language skills (Sihvonen et al., 2021; c.f., Tuncer et al., 2021). Direct evidence for the importance of FAT in speech production comes from intraoperative stimulation of the tract, which often causes speech arrest (Vassal et al., 2014; Fujii et al., 2015; Kinoshita et al., 2015) or stuttering (Kemerdere et al., 2016). Post-operative damage to FAT can cause a transient problem of initiating spontaneous speech, although this problem often resolves itself rapidly and almost completely (Vassal et al., 2014; Fujii et al., 2015; Kinoshita et al., 2015; Young et al., 2020).

More specifically, a link has been established between fluency in speech production and FAT using different techniques and populations. In a study of 35 individuals with PPA, Catani et al. (2013) did not find a general correlation between FAT measures and overall language impairment, grammatical impairment, repetition, or word comprehension, but they reported that FAT abnormalities are particularly correlated with agrammatic PPA. Moreover, FA of FAT was positively correlated with fluency, measured as words per minute, and mean length of utterance, while radial diffusivity showed an inverse correlation with the same two measures (see also Mandelli et al., 2014). Similarly, in a study of 46 chronic post-stroke individuals, Alyahya et al. (2020) found that the properties of FAT and the anterior parts of AF were predictive of both the quantity and quality of the connected speech (see also Basilakos et al., 2014; Halai et al., 2017; Ille et al., 2018). In yet another population, Chenausky et al. (2017) tested 10 minimally verbal children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), and found a correlation between FA in AF and FAT in the percentage of correct syllable-initial consonants and percentage of syllable-insertion errors, respectively.

FAT abnormalities have also been linked to stuttering (Kronfeld-Duenias et al., 2016). The nature of the findings has been different though. While Kornfeld-Duenias and colleagues found different diffusivity rates in the left FAT to be predictive of stuttering, Neef et al. (2018) found stronger connectivity of the right FAT to be predictive of more severe stuttering. Consequently, the interpretations were different: the former highlighted the critical importance of left FAT for linguistic sequencing, while the latter took their findings to imply an amplification of the function of the right IFG, namely enforcing global inhibition. While a contribution is clear, more work is needed to understand the role of the left vs. right FAT in stuttering. More recently, FAT has been linked to articulatory-motor planning (Faulkner and Wilshire, 2020; although the use of a probabilistic map in that study makes tract localization less reliable). As noted in the earlier sections, disentangling phonological and post-phonological processes in language production can be tricky, but at least one study that has specifically tested the contribution of FAT to speech apraxia in 52 stroke survivors found apraxia to be associated with lesions to the pre- and post-central gyri and the left dorsal AF but not with FAT (Chenausky et al., 2020).

It is worth mentioning that the FAT is often implicated in verbal fluency tasks along with other tracts, especially IFOF, although the evidence is, as in the other cases, far from convergent. Some studies have reported a correlation with both category and letter fluency scores (Li et al., 2017; Blecher et al., 2019; Costentin et al., 2019; Sanvito et al., 2020), some only with letter/phoneme fluency (Cipolotti et al., 2016; Keser et al., 2020), some with morpheme-based fluency (Yablonski et al., 2021) and some with none of these (Tseng et al., 2019; Vallesi and Babcock, 2020). Costentin et al. (2019) found a correlation between verbal fluency scores and lesions to FAT and a number of other tracts in 48 individuals with Parkinson’s Disease. However, the decline in performance in these tasks after surgery was not correlated with the proportion of the fibers or the number of tracts disconnected. In short, the same heterogeneity observed in the correspondence between verbal fluency scores and some of the other white matter tracts is evident here as well.

Moreover, and similar to the SLF and AF, a syntactic function has also been proposed for FAT, mainly due to its connection to IFG, which has, by some, been proposed as a critical region for syntactic production (e.g., Friederici et al., 2003). The evidence for this link, however, is not watertight. For example, the evidence linking agrammatic aphasia to FAT abnormalities (e.g., Catani et al., 2013) is often confounded with other problems, e.g., phonological planning for longer utterances and higher demands on working memory. Incidentally, some researchers have linked bilateral FAT to working memory in older adults (Rizio and Diaz, 2016). A study that is often taken as a clear evidence in favor of a syntactic role for FAT is the study of Sierpowska et al. (2015). These authors reported a case of intraoperative stimulation of the left FAT, where the patient showed a selective deficit for generating verbs by adding morphemes to nouns (e.g., book → booked) instead of producing the verb usually associated with the noun (book → read). This was taken as a marker of a deficit specific to morphological processing. However, the task has a strong cognitive control demand (rejecting a high-frequency, strongly associated verb in favor of a lower-frequency, weakly associated alternative), typical for left IFG recruitment (e.g., Thompson-Schill et al., 1997). It is thus possible that FAT’s role is related to conflict resolution.

In line with this hypothesis are two other reports of intraoperative stimulations, by Chernoff et al. (2018) and Dragoy et al. (2020). In the former study, the authors reported a patient with surgical damage to the connectivity of the left FAT, who showed a selective post-surgical impairment of fluency in the form of difficulty with voluntary speech and more complex sequences, while remaining unimpaired in production tasks such as picture naming and auditory repetition. In the latter, Dragoy and colleagues reported that cortical stimulation of the termination points of the left FAT (superiorly in the SMA and pre-SMA and inferiorly in the pars triangularis and opercularis of IFG) caused selective impairment in a sentence completion task with low close probability (e.g., sentence prompts such as “A piggy is chewing…” which can be completed with a number of different words). Past research has shown a similar involvement of the IFG (and potentially ACC, SMA, and pre-SMA) in high-conflict tasks and those with under-determined responses, such as spontaneous speech or completing sentences with many possible endings (e.g., Robinson et al., 2010).

These results bring up the question of whether the FAT is truly involved in “syntactic” processing or rather in conflict resolution operations that are often required for the processing of syntactically complex structures. Chernoff et al. (2019) proposed an alternative syntactic function for the tract, namely, sequencing complex actions with a hierarchical structure. In a case study of a patient undergoing awake craniotomy for removing a left frontal tumor, the authors showed that electrical stimulation of the left FAT affected pauses at the beginning of grammatical phrases without influencing either word durations or the durations of noun phrases. This is a neat and informative finding, but it does not necessarily mean involvement in any complex syntactic function. In section 2, under Articulatory processing, we reviewed the role of the planning loop, consisting of pre-SMA and left pIFS, which buffers utterances before it is time to send them to the SMA, vPMC, and ultimately vMC for articulation. It is reasonable to assume that buffering happens at the level of short grammatical phrases, and to the extent that FAT is involved in the transmission of these buffered chunks, it is expected that the stimulation effects should manifest at phrasal boundaries. But note that this is a lower-level sequencing operation for motor execution, rather than a higher-level syntactic operation, as in generating a hierarchical syntactic structure per se.

A closer look at the right FAT may help adjudicate some of these competing representations. The Right IFG (rIFG) has long been proposed as a critical pathway in “stopping” behavior (Aron et al., 2003, 2004; Aron, 2007). Through the direct pathway, rIFG activates the subthalamic nucleus to enforce stopping (Aron and Poldrack, 2006; Cai and Leung, 2009; Favre et al., 2013; Jahanshahi, 2013; Wiecki and Frank, 2013; Obeso et al., 2014; van Wouwe et al., 2017). There is now evidence that pre-SMA may be a part of this pathway (Nachev et al., 2008; Aron et al., 2016). The right pre-SMA is usually more activated during successful than unsuccessful stops (Aron and Poldrack, 2006; Aron, 2007; Boehler et al., 2010). Its lesions cause a deficit in the execution of complex motor movements, especially in the presence of competing action plans (Nachev et al., 2007), and its direct stimulation stops ongoing movement (Lüders et al., 1988; Mikuni et al., 2006). Whether specialized for “stopping” or rather, context monitoring (Hampshire et al., 2010; Chatham et al., 2012; Erika-Florence et al., 2014; Hampshire, 2015), the evidence strongly points to the involvement of these regions, and their connecting fiber right FAT, in inhibitory control of behavior. Given this evidence, and the symmetry of the cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic-cerebellar circuits in the two hemispheres, Dick et al. (2019) proposed that the FAT is involved in the same function, namely selecting the appropriate plan for motor actions among competing alternatives, in both hemispheres. On the left side, this function is primarily—but not exclusively (Budisavljevic et al., 2017)—applied to language (see Nozari and Hepner, 2019a,b, for a potential application to decisions regarding stopping or proceeding in language production). On the right side, the function applies more broadly to the action domain, especially in visuo-motor tasks. Such a gating function is highly appealing from the perspective of domain-general computations applied to domain-specific representations (e.g., Middleton and Strick, 2000; Hepner and Nozari, 2019), and is a promising framework for future studies of FAT.

11.3. Summary

Due to its links to IFG, pre-SMA, and SMA, the FAT is a good candidate for implementing domain-general functions that also apply to language processing. Although both motor and syntactic functions have been proposed, a broader consideration of the role of the regions connected by this tract implicates it in monitoring and control processes that, especially on the left side, regulate the chunking and outputting of articulatory segments. Future work can further test the scope and limits of these functions in FAT.

12. Summary and recommendations for future directions

Table 1 provides a summary of the language-related functions attributed to the eight major tracts reviewed above. Generally speaking, the operations linked to the tracts reviewed in the earlier sections of this article are compatible with the architecture of the dual-stream model. The ventral tracts are involved primarily in mapping the auditory input to lexical and ultimately semantic representations in comprehension, as well as mapping semantic concepts onto lexical representations in production. On the other hand, the dorsal tracts are primarily involved in more distal operations in the production pathway, i.e., mapping phonological representations to articulatory plans, buffering of those plans, and mapping them onto their corresponding articulatory motor outputs. These tracts also carry out the mapping between auditory representations and the production chain mentioned above and are thus critical in auditory repetition tasks. Past this rough characterization, however, there are several as-of-yet-unknown details. Below, we discuss some of the outstanding issues (Box 1), and some recommendations for addressing them in future research.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of the language-related functions attributed to the eight major tracts reviewed in this article.

Box 1. Outstanding questions for future research.

Ventral tracts

(1)  Are some tracts specialized for comprehension vs. production?

(2)  Are some tracts specialized for semantic control?

Dorsal tracts

(1)  Which tracts are specialized for phonological buffering in working memory?

(2)  What is the role of FAT?

Ventral and dorsal tracts

(1)  Which tracts are involved in which aspects of reading?

(2)  Which tracts are involved in which aspects of syntactic processing?

12.1. Ventral stream

Two outstanding questions are (1) Are some tracts specialized for comprehension vs. production?; and (2) Are some tracts specialized for semantic control as opposed to semantic activation? Currently, the empirical data are not conclusive on these two points. Although there is some evidence that IFOF and ILF may be more involved in production and perception, respectively, this distinction is not uncontested. The evidence linking individual tracts to semantic-lexical control is even less consistent, with ILF, IFOF, and UF each implicated in some, but not in other, studies. Thus, future research on ventral tracts can benefit from cognitive tasks that: (a) compare production vs. comprehension using a similar set of materials, e.g., picture naming vs. picture-word matching in individuals with brain damage; and (b) compare production/perception in conditions with low control demands to those with high control demands. An example in production is picture naming in the context of other semantically related vs. unrelated items (Costa et al., 2006; Schnur et al., 2009; Nozari et al., 2016b; Hepner and Nozari, 2020; McDonagh et al., 2020). The comprehension equivalent is picture-word matching with semantically related vs. unrelated distractors (e.g., Nozari, 2019). Although some of these tasks have been used in some studies, there is a clear need for more studies with larger sample sizes, larger sets, and better-controlled materials to reconcile some of the discrepancies in the existing findings.

12.2. Dorsal stream

Two outstanding questions here concern the differentiation in the role of the tracts involved in phonological processing, and the role of FAT. We unpack each question below: (1) Which tracts are involved in pure mapping of phonological representations to more peripheral representations, and which tracts are critical in the working memory (i.e., buffering) operations involving phonological representations. This distinction can be behaviorally tested by comparing the production of shorter vs. longer words (which require greater buffering; e.g., Goldrick and Rapp, 2007), or other tests of phonological working memory, although preferably those that do not pose additional demands on conflict resolution, such as discriminating between close phonological alternatives in working memory. Some researchers have proposed that the same regions storing phonological representations are also involved in buffering them (Acheson et al., 2010), while others have suggested separate neural regions specialized for phonological buffering (Yue et al., 2018). The identification of the white matter tracts selectively involved in phonological working memory may shed some light on this debate. (2) The role of FAT is also still debated, with the proposal of a domain-general conflict-resolution and selection function, with some degree of domain-specificity for language in the left hemisphere, as the most promising theoretical framework for designing empirical studies.

12.3. Ventral and dorsal streams

Two sets of operations have been frequently suggested for tracts in both streams, reading and syntactic production. It is not surprising that reading has its signature over both tracts, as it can encompass a wide range of operations including the retrieval of phonological, lexical, and semantic representations, conflict resolution for visually, phonologically, or semantically similar items, and in some cases even activating motor commands. Most studies that link reading to different tracts have not disentangled different aspects of reading. Similarly, syntax encompasses a wide range of operations, some but not all of which require maintaining long-distance dependencies and reconciling competition between alternative representations, which may reflect more domain-general abilities rather than syntactic processes per se (e.g., Nozari and Omaki, 2022). Most studies of syntactic processing have not attempted to carefully disentangle these facets of processing.

In short, two questions are outstanding here: (1) Which tracts are involved in which aspect(s) of reading? More data from studies that differentiate the contribution of lexical and sublexical reading inspired by cognitive theories (e.g., Coltheart et al., 2001) would shed light on this question. (2) Which tracts are involved in which aspect(s) of syntactic processing? A more systematic study of syntactic processing by carving it at its joints is likely to yield more informative results. One proposed dimension is Matchin et al.’s (2017) separation of syntactic operations in comprehension vs. production. But there are many other possible divisions (or subdivisions within the comprehension/production framework) that can further shed light on the role of different neural regions in various aspects of syntactic processing. For example, one could test whether morphological processing can be disentangled from working memory processes which mediate the relationship between two parts of a sentence with dependencies.

13. Conclusion

Much has already been learned about the network involved in processing language production and comprehension, including the white matter pathways that connect various cortical regions. The field has moved beyond the general question of whether a tract is or is not involved in language processing and has reached a state of probing the nuanced nature of such involvement. This is an excellent time for combining theoretically inspired approaches with neural investigations. Specifically, the review above shows the need for moving away from paradigms that confound multiple operations, e.g., verbal fluency tasks, and towards those that can better tease apart cognitive components such as semantic activation vs. semantic control, phonological activation vs. phonological buffering, and pure syntactic operations vs. domain-general processes that support such operations.

Author contributions

ES contributed to conceptualizing the work, reviewing the literature, generating the table and figures, and writing the masnucript. NN contributed to conceptuliaizing and writing the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Shima Dorafshan for her help with preparing the figures.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Acheson, D. J., Postle, B. R., and Macdonald, M. C. (2010). The interaction of concreteness and phonological similarity in verbal working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 36, 17–36. doi: 10.1037/a0017679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agosta, F., Galantucci, S., Canu, E., Cappa, S. F., Magnani, G., Franceschi, M., et al. (2013). Disruption of structural connectivity along the dorsal and ventral language pathways in patients with nonfluent and semantic variant primary progressive aphasia: a DT MRI study and a literature review. Brain Lang. 127, 157–166. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agosta, F., Scola, E., Canu, E., Marcone, A., Magnani, G., Sarro, L., et al. (2012). White matter damage in frontotemporal lobar degeneration spectrum. Cereb. Cortex 22, 2705–2714. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, A. L., Lee, J. E., Lazar, M., and Field, A. S. (2007). Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics 4, 316–329. doi: 10.1016/j.nurt.2007.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allendorfer, J. B., Hernando, K. A., Hossain, S., Nenert, R., Holland, S. K., and Szaflarski, J. P. (2016). Arcuate fasciculus asymmetry has a hand in language function but not handedness. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 3297–3309. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alm, K. H., Rolheiser, T., and Olson, I. R. (2016). Inter-individual variation in fronto-temporal connectivity predicts the ability to learn different types of associations. Neuroimage 132, 213–224. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Almairac, F., Herbet, G., Moritz-Gasser, S., de Champfleur, N. M., and Duffau, H. (2015). The left inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus subserves language semantics: a multilevel lesion study. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 1983–1995. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0773-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alyahya, R. S. W., Halai, A. D., Conroy, P., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2020). A unified model of post-stroke language deficits including discourse production and their neural correlates. Brain 143, 1541–1554. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ameis, S. H., Fan, J., Rockel, C., Voineskos, A. N., Lobaugh, N. J., Soorya, L., et al. (2011). Impaired structural connectivity of socio-emotional circuits in autism spectrum disorders: a diffusion tensor imaging study. PLoS One 6:e28044. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, J., and Nozari, N. (2017). The effects of utterance timing and stimulation of left prefrontal cortex on the production of referential expressions. Cognition 160, 127–144. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist 13, 214–228. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., Behrens, T. E., Smith, S., Frank, M. J., and Poldrack, R. A. (2007). Triangulating a cognitive control network using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI. J. Neurosci. 27, 3743–3752. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0519-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, E. T., Sahakian, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2003). Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 115–116. doi: 10.1038/nn1003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., Herz, D. M., Brown, P., Forstmann, B. U., and Zaghloul, K. (2016). Frontosubthalamic circuits for control of action and cognition. J. Neurosci. 36, 11489–11495. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2348-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., Monsell, S., Sahakian, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2004). A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. Brain 127, 1561–1573. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., and Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Cortical and subcortical contributions to Stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J. Neurosci. 26, 2424–2433. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4682-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arrington, N. C., Kulesz, P. A., Juranek, J., Cirino, P. T., and Fletcher, J. M. (2017). White matter microstructure integrity in relation to reading proficiency. Brain Lang. 174, 103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asaridou, S. S., Demir-Lira, Ö. E., Goldin-Meadow, S., and Small, S. L. (2017). The pace of vocabulary growth during preschool predicts cortical structure at school age. Neuropsychologia 98, 13–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axer, H., Klingner, C. M., and Prescher, A. (2013). Fiber anatomy of dorsal and ventral language streams. Brain Lang. 127, 192–204. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baggio, H. C., Segura, B., Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N., Valldeoriola, F., Marti, M. J., Compta, Y., et al. (2012). Structural correlates of facial emotion recognition deficits in PD’s disease patients. Neuropsychologia 50, 2121–2128. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakhtiari, R., Boliek, C., and Cummine, J. (2014). Investigating the contribution of ventral-lexical and dorsal-sublexical pathways during reading in bilinguals. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:507. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldo, J. V., and Dronkers, N. F. (2006). The role of inferior parietal and inferior frontal cortex in working memory. Neuropsychology 20, 529–538. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.5.529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldo, J. V., and Dronkers, N. F. (2007). Neural correlates of arithmetic and language comprehension: a common substrate. Neuropsychologia 45, 229–235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbeau, E. B., Descoteaux, M., and Petrides, M. (2020). Dissociating the white matter tracts connecting the temporo-parietal cortical region with frontal cortex using diffusion tractography. Sci. Rep. 10:8186. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-64124-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basilakos, A., Fillmore, P. T., Rorden, C., Guo, D., Bonilha, L., and Fridriksson, J. (2014). Regional white matter damage predicts speech fluency in chronic post-stroke aphasia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:845. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basser, P. J., Mattiello, J., and LeBihan, D. (1994). MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophys. J. 66, 259–267. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(94)80775-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, E., Wilson, S. M., Saygin, A. P., Dick, F., Sereno, M. I., Knight, R. T., et al. (2003). Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 448–450. doi: 10.1038/nn1050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baur, V., Hänggi, J., and Jäncke, L. (2012). Volumetric associations between uncinate fasciculus, amygdala and trait anxiety. BMC Neurosci. 13:4. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benson, D. F., Sheremata, W. A., Bouchard, R., Segarra, J. M., Price, D., and Geschwind, N. (1973). Conduction aphasia: a clinicopathological study. Arch. Neurol. 28, 339–346. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1973.00490230075011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernard, F., Zemmoura, I., Ter Minassian, A., Lemée, J.-M., and Menei, P. (2019). Anatomical variability of the arcuate fasciculus: a systematical review. Surg. Radiol. Anat. 41, 889–900. doi: 10.1007/s00276-019-02244-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berwick, R. C., Friederici, A. D., Chomsky, N., and Bolhuis, J. J. (2013). Evolution, brain and the nature of language. Trends Cognit. Sci. 17, 89–98. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, J. R. (2007). “Effects of word imageability on semantic access: neuroimaging studies,” in Neural Basis of Semantic Memory, Eds. J. Hart and M. A. Kraut (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 149–181.

Binder, J. R. (2015). The wernicke area: modern evidence and a reinterpretation. Neurology 85, 2170–2175. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, J. R., and Desai, R. H. (2011). The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends Cognit. Sci. 15, 527–536. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., and Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? a critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2767–2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blecher, T., Miron, S., Schneider, G. G., Achiron, A., and Ben-Shachar, M. (2019). Association between white matter microstructure and verbal fluency in patients with multiple sclerosis. Front. Psychol. 10:1607. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blom-Smink, M., Verly, M., Spielmann, K., Smits, M., Ribbers, G. M., and van de Sandt-Koenderman, M. W. M. E. (2020). Change in right inferior longitudinal fasciculus integrity is associated with naming recovery in subacute poststroke aphasia. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 34, 784–794. doi: 10.1177/1545968320940982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blumstein, S. E., Myers, E. B., and Rissman, J. (2005). The perception of voice onset time: an fMRI investigation of phonetic category structure. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 17, 1353–1366. doi: 10.1162/0898929054985473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boehler, C. N., Appelbaum, L. G., Krebs, R. M., Hopf, J.-M., and Woldorff, M. G. (2010). Pinning down response inhibition in the brain—conjunction analyses of the stop-signal task. Neuroimage 52, 1621–1632. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohland, J. W., Bullock, D., and Guenther, F. H. (2010). Neural representations and mechanisms for the performance of simple speech sequences. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 22, 1504–1529. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borchers, L. R., Bruckert, L., Dodson, C. K., Travis, K. E., Marchman, V. A., Ben-Shachar, M., et al. (2019). Microstructural properties of white matter pathways in relation to subsequent reading abilities in children: a longitudinal analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 891–905. doi: 10.1007/s00429-018-1813-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., Schlesewsky, M., Small, S. L., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2015). Neurobiological roots of language in primate audition: common computational properties. Trends Cognit. Sci. 19, 142–150. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botha, H., Duffy, J. R., Whitwell, J. L., Strand, E. A., Machulda, M. M., Schwarz, C. G., et al. (2015). Classification and clinicoradiologic features of primary progressive aphasia (PPA) and apraxia of speech. Cortex 69, 220–236. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouchard, L.-O., Wilson, M. A., Laforce, R., and Duchesne, S. (2019). White matter damage in the semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 46, 373–382. doi: 10.1017/cjn.2019.37

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bozkurt, B., Yagmurlu, K., Middlebrooks, E. H., Karadag, A., Ovalioglu, T. C., Jagadeesan, B., et al. (2016). Microsurgical and tractographic anatomy of the supplementary motor area complex in humans. World Neurosurg. 95, 99–107. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2016.07.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brauer, J., Anwander, A., Perani, D., and Friederici, A. D. (2013). Dorsal and ventral pathways in language development. Brain Lang. 127, 289–295. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breier, J. I., Hasan, K. M., Zhang, W., Men, D., and Papanicolaou, A. C. (2008). Language dysfunction after stroke and damage to white matter tracts evaluated using diffusion tensor imaging. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 29, 483–487. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A0846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briggs, R. G., Rahimi, M., Conner, A. K., Sali, G., Baker, C. M., Burks, J. D., et al. (2018a). A connectomic atlas of the human cerebrum—chapter 15: tractographic description of the uncinate fasciculus. Oper. Neurosurg. (Hagerstown) 15, S450–S455. doi: 10.1093/ons/opy269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briggs, R. G., Conner, A. K., Rahimi, M., Sali, G., Baker, C. M., Burks, J. D., et al. (2018b). A connectomic atlas of the human cerebrum—chapter 14: tractographic description of the frontal aslant tract. Oper. Neurosurg. (Hagerstown) 15, S444–S449. doi: 10.1093/ons/opy268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broce, I. J., Bernal, B., Altman, N., Bradley, C., Baez, N., Cabrera, L., et al. (2019). Fiber pathways supporting early literacy development in 5-8-year-old children. Brain Cognit. 134, 80–89. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broce, I., Bernal, B., Altman, N., Tremblay, P., and Dick, A. S. (2015). Fiber tracking of the frontal aslant tract and subcomponents of the arcuate fasciculus in 5-8-year-olds: relation to speech and language function. Brain Lang. 149, 66–76. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruckert, L., Borchers, L. R., Dodson, C. K., Marchman, V. A., Travis, K. E., Ben-Shachar, M., et al. (2019). White matter plasticity in reading-related pathways differs in children born preterm and at term: a longitudinal analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:139. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchsbaum, B. R., and D’Esposito, M. (2008). Repetition suppression and reactivation in auditory-verbal short-term recognition memory. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1474–1485. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Budisavljevic, S., Dell’Acqua, F., Djordjilovic, V., Miotto, D., Motta, R., and Castiello, U. (2017). The role of the frontal aslant tract and premotor connections in visually guided hand movements. Neuroimage 146, 419–428. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullock, D., Takemura, H., Caiafa, C. F., Kitchell, L., McPherson, B., Caron, B., et al. (2019). Associative white matter connecting the dorsal and ventral posterior human cortex. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 2631–2660. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01907-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burdach, K. F. (1826). Vom Baue und Leben des Gehirns. Leipzig, Germany: In der Dyk’schen Buchhandlung.

Bürgel, U., Amunts, K., Hoemke, L., Mohlberg, H., Gilsbach, J. M., and Zilles, K. (2006). White matter fiber tracts of the human brain: Three-dimensional mapping at microscopic resolution, topography and intersubject variability. Neuroimage 29, 1092–1105. doi: 10.1093/ofid/ofad070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burks, J. D., Boettcher, L. B., Conner, A. K., Glenn, C. A., Bonney, P. A., Baker, C. M., et al. (2017). White matter connections of the inferior parietal lobule: a study of surgical anatomy. Brain Behav. 7:e00640. doi: 10.1002/brb3.640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burns, J., Job, D., Bastin, M. E., Whalley, H., Macgillivray, T., Johnstone, E. C., et al. (2003). Structural disconnectivity in schizophrenia: a diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging study. Br. J. Psychiatry 182, 439–443. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.5.439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, W., and Leung, H.-C. (2009). Cortical activity during manual response inhibition guided by color and orientation cues. Brain Res. 1261, 20–28. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.12.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caramazza, A., and Zurif, E. B. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: evidence from aphasia. Brain Lang. 3, 572–582. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(76)90048-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, J. C., Lanham, D. C., Cutting, L. E., Clements-Stephens, A. M., Chen, X., Hadzipasic, M., et al. (2009). A dual DTI approach to analyzing white matter in children with dyslexia. Psychiatry Res. 172, 215–219. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catani, M., Dell’Acqua, F., Vergani, F., Malik, F., Hodge, H., Roy, P., et al. (2012). Short frontal lobe connections of the human brain. Cortex 48, 273–291. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catani, M., Howard, R. J., Pajevic, S., and Jones, D. K. (2002). Virtual in vivo interactive dissection of white matter fasciculi in the human brain. Neuroimage 17, 77–94. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catani, M., Jones, D. K., Donato, R., and ffytche, D. H. (2003). Occipito-temporal connections in the human brain. Brain 126, 2093–2107. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catani, M., Jones, D. K., and ffytche, D. H. (2005). Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Ann. Neurol. 57, 8–16. doi: 10.1002/ana.20319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catani, M., Mesulam, M. M., Jakobsen, E., Malik, F., Martersteck, A., Wieneke, C., et al. (2013). A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal fluency in primary progressive aphasia. Brain 136, 2619–2628. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catani, M., and Thiebaut de Schotten, M. (2008). A diffusion tensor imaging tractography atlas for virtual in vivo dissections. Cortex 44, 1105–1132. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caverzasi, E., Hervey-Jumper, S. L., Jordan, K. M., Lobach, I. V., Li, J., Panara, V., et al. (2016). Identifying preoperative language tracts and predicting postoperative functional recovery using HARDI q-ball fiber tractography in patients with gliomas. J. Neurosurg. 125, 33–45. doi: 10.3171/2015.6.JNS142203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caverzasi, E., Papinutto, N., Amirbekian, B., Berger, M. S., and Henry, R. G. (2014). Q-ball of inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus and beyond. PLoS One 9:e100274. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0100274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, S. E., Zhu, D. C., Choo, A. L., and Angstadt, M. (2015). White matter neuroanatomical differences in young children who stutter. Brain 138, 694–711. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatham, C. H., Claus, E. D., Kim, A., Curran, T., Banich, M. T., and Munakata, Y. (2012). Cognitive control reflects context monitoring, not motoric stopping, in response inhibition. PLoS One 7:e31546. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, L., Wassermann, D., Abrams, D. A., Kochalka, J., Gallardo-Diez, G., and Menon, V. (2019). The visual word form area (VWFA) is part of both language and attention circuitry. Nat. Commun. 10:5601. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13634-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chenausky, K., Kernbach, J., Norton, A., and Schlaug, G. (2017). White matter integrity and treatment-based change in speech performance in minimally verbal children with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:175. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chenausky, K., Paquette, S., Norton, A., and Schlaug, G. (2020). Apraxia of speech involves lesions of dorsal arcuate fasciculus and insula in patients with aphasia. Neurol. Clin. Pract. 10, 162–169. doi: 10.1212/CPJ.0000000000000699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chernoff, B. L., Sims, M. H., Smith, S. O., Pilcher, W. H., and Mahon, B. Z. (2019). Direct electrical stimulation of the left frontal aslant tract disrupts sentence planning without affecting articulation. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 36, 178–192. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2019.1619544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chernoff, B. L., Teghipco, A., Garcea, F. E., Sims, M. H., Paul, D. A., Tivarus, M. E., et al. (2018). A role for the frontal aslant tract in speech planning: a neurosurgical case study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 752–769. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christidi, F., Zalonis, I., Kyriazi, S., Rentzos, M., Karavasilis, E., Wilde, E. A., et al. (2014). Uncinate fasciculus microstructure and verbal episodic memory in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a diffusion tensor imaging and neuropsychological study. Brain Imag. Behav. 8, 497–505. doi: 10.1007/s11682-013-9271-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cipolotti, L., Spanò, B., Healy, C., Tudor-Sfetea, C., Chan, E., White, M., et al. (2016). Inhibition processes are dissociable and lateralized in human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 93, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coad, B. M., Postans, M., Hodgetts, C. J., Muhlert, N., Graham, K. S., and Lawrence, A. D. (2020). Structural connections support emotional connections: uncinate fasciculus microstructure is related to the ability to decode facial emotion expressions. Neuropsychologia 145:106562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cocquyt, E. M., Lanckmans, E., van Mierlo, P., Duyck, W., Szmalec, A., Santens, P., et al. (2020). The white matter architecture underlying semantic processing: a systematic review. Neuropsychologia 136:107182. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coello, A. F., Duvaux, S., De Benedictis, A., Matsuda, R., and Duffau, H. (2013). Involvement of the right inferior longitudinal fascicle in visual hemiagnosia: a brain stimulation mapping study: case report. J. Neurosurg. 118, 202–205. doi: 10.3171/2012.10.JNS12527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., and Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychol. Rev. 108, 204–256. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.1.204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conner, A. K., Briggs, R. G., Sali, G., Rahimi, M., Baker, C. M., Burks, J. D., et al. (2018a). A connectomic atlas of the human cerebrum—chapter 13: tractographic description of the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus. Oper. Neurosurg. (Hagerstown) 15, S436–S443. doi: 10.1093/ons/opy267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conner, A. K., Briggs, R. G., Rahimi, M., Sali, G., Baker, C. M., Burks, J. D., et al. (2018b). A connectomic atlas of the human cerebrum—chapter 12: tractographic description of the middle longitudinal fasciculus. Oper. Neurosurg. (Hagerstown) 15, S429–S435. doi: 10.1093/ons/opy266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, A., Santesteban, M., and Ivanova, I. (2006). How do highly proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 32, 1057–1074. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, A., Strijkers, K., Martin, C., and Thierry, G. (2009). The time course of word retrieval revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 21442–21446. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908921106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costentin, G., Derrey, S., Gérardin, E., Cruypeninck, Y., Pressat-Laffouilhere, T., Anouar, Y., et al. (2019). White matter tracts lesions and decline of verbal fluency after deep brain stimulation in PD’s disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 2561–2570. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowan, W. M., Gottlieb, D. I., Hendrickson, A. E., Price, J. L., and Woolsey, T. A. (1972). The autoradiographic demonstration of axonal connections in the central nervous system. Brain Res. 37, 21–51. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90344-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craddock, R. C., Jbabdi, S., Yan, C.-G., Vogelstein, J. T., Castellanos, F. X., Di Martino, A., et al. (2013). Imaging human connectomes at the macroscale. Nat. Methods 10, 524–539. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummine, J., Dai, W., Borowsky, R., Gould, L., Rollans, C., and Boliek, C. (2015). Investigating the ventral-lexical, dorsal-sublexical model of basic reading processes using diffusion tensor imaging. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 445–455. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0666-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Anna, L., Mesulam, M. M., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Dell’Acqua, F., Murphy, D., Wieneke, C., et al. (2016). Frontotemporal networks and behavioral symptoms in primary progressive aphasia. Neurology 86, 1393–1399. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dávolos, J. M., Arias, J. C., and Jefferies, E. (2020). Linking individual differences in semantic cognition to white matter microstructure. Neuropsychologia 141:107438. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Erausquin, G., and Alba-Ferrara, L. (2013). What does anisotropy measure? Insights from increased and decreased anisotropy in selective fiber tracts in schizophrenia. Front. Integrat. Neurosci. 7:9. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Witt Hamer, P. C., Moritz-Gasser, S., Gatignol, P., and Duffau, H. (2011). Is the human left middle longitudinal fascicle essential for language? a brain electrostimulation study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 962–973. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Zubicaray, G. I., Rose, S. E., and McMahon, K. L. (2011). The structure and connectivity of semantic memory in the healthy older adult brain. Neuroimage 54, 1488–1494. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dejerine, J. (1895). Anatomie Des Centres Nerveux (Vol. 1). Rueff et Cie. Available online at: https://gallica.bnf.fr/ark:/12148/bpt6k76498h/f7.item.

Google Scholar

Del Tufo, S. N., Earle, F. S., and Cutting, L. E. (2019). The impact of expressive language development and the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus on listening and reading comprehension. J. Neurodev. Disord. 11:37. doi: 10.1186/s11689-019-9296-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychol. Rev. 93, 283–321.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dell, G. S., Nozari, N., and Oppenheim, G. M. (2014). “Word production: behavioral and computational considerations,” in The Oxford Handbook of Language Production, Eds. M. A. Goldrick, V. Ferreira, and M. Miozzo (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 88–104).

Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Nozari, N., Faseyitan, O., and Branch Coslett, H. (2013). Voxel-based lesion-parameter mapping: Identifying the neural correlates of a computational model of word production. Cognition 128, 380–396. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derrfuss, J., Brass, M., and Yves von Cramon, D. (2004). Cognitive control in the posterior frontolateral cortex: evidence from common activations in task coordination, interference control and working memory. Neuroimage 23, 604–612. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Tella, S., Baglio, F., Pelizzari, L., Cabinio, M., Nemni, R., Traficante, D., et al. (2020). Uncinate fasciculus and word selection processing in PD’s disease. Neuropsychologia 146:107504. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dick, A. S., Bernal, B., and Tremblay, P. (2014). The language connectome:new pathways, new concepts. Neuroscientist 20, 453–467. doi: 10.1177/1073858413513502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dick, A. S., Garic, D., Graziano, P., and Tremblay, P. (2019). The frontal aslant tract (FAT) and its role in speech, language and executive function. Cortex 111, 148–163. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dick, A. S., and Tremblay, P. (2012). Beyond the arcuate fasciculus: consensus and controversy in the connectional anatomy of language. Brain 135, 3529–3550. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diehl, B., Busch, R. M., Duncan, J. S., Piao, Z., Tkach, J., and Lüders, H. O. (2008). Abnormalities in diffusion tensor imaging of the uncinate fasciculus relate to reduced memory in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 49, 1409–1418. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2008.01596.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodson, C. K., Travis, K. E., Borchers, L. R., Marchman, V. A., Ben-Shachar, M., and Feldman, H. M. (2018). White matter properties associated with pre-reading skills in 6-year-old children born preterm and at term. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 60, 695–702. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dragoy, O., Zyryanov, A., Bronov, O., Gordeyeva, E., Gronskaya, N., Kryuchkova, O., et al. (2020). Functional linguistic specificity of the left frontal aslant tract for spontaneous speech fluency: evidence from intraoperative language mapping. Brain Lang. 208:104836. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2020.104836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D. P., Van Valin, R. D., Redfern, B. B., and Jaeger, J. J. (2004). Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition 92, 145–177. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffau, H. (2015). Stimulation mapping of white matter tracts to study brain functional connectivity. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 11, 255–265. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.51

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffau, H., Capelle, L., Denvil, D., Gatignol, P., Sichez, N., Lopes, M., et al. (2003). The role of dominant premotor cortex in language: a study using intraoperative functional mapping in awake patients. Neuroimage 20, 1903–1914. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00203-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffau, H., Capelle, L., Sichez, N., Denvil, D., Lopes, M., Sichez, J. P., et al. (2002). Intraoperative mapping of the subcortical language pathways using direct stimulations: an anatomo-functional study. Brain 125, 199–214. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffau, H., Gatignol, P., Mandonnet, E., Capelle, L., and Taillandier, L. (2008). Intraoperative subcortical stimulation mapping of language pathways in a consecutive series of 115 patients with Grade II glioma in the left dominant hemisphere. J. Neurosurg. 109, 461–471. doi: 10.3171/JNS/2008/109/9/0461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffau, H., Gatignol, P., Mandonnet, E., Peruzzi, P., Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., and Capelle, L. (2005). New insights into the anatomo-functional connectivity of the semantic system: a study using cortico-subcortical electrostimulations. Brain 128, 797–810. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffau, H., Gatignol, P., Moritz-Gasser, S., and Mandonnet, E. (2009). Is the left uncinate fasciculus essential for language? a cerebral stimulation study. J. Neurol. 256, 382–389. doi: 10.1007/s00415-009-0053-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffau, H., Moritz-Gasser, S., and Mandonnet, E. (2014). A re-examination of neural basis of language processing: proposal of a dynamic hodotopical model from data provided by brain stimulation mapping during picture naming. Brain Lang. 131, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebeling, U., and Cramon, D. v. (1992). Topography of the uncinate fascicle and adjacent temporal fiber tracts. Acta Neurochirurgica 115, 143–148. doi: 10.1007/BF01406373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Efthymiopoulou, E., Kasselimis, D. S., Ghika, A., Kyrozis, A., Peppas, C., Evdokimidis, I., et al. (2017). The effect of cortical and subcortical lesions on spontaneous expression of memory-encoded and emotionally infused information: evidence for a role of the ventral stream. Neuropsychologia 101, 115–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eichert, N., Verhagen, L., Folloni, D., Jbabdi, S., Khrapitchev, A. A., Sibson, N. R., et al. (2019). What is special about the human arcuate fasciculus? Lateralization, projections and expansion. Cortex 118, 107–115. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enatsu, R., Kanno, A., Ookawa, S., Ochi, S., Ishiai, S., Nagamine, T., et al. (2017). Distribution and network of basal temporal language areas: a study of the combination of electric cortical stimulation and diffusion tensor imaging. World Neurosurg. 106, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2017.06.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epelbaum, S., Pinel, P., Gaillard, R., Delmaire, C., Perrin, M., Dupont, S., et al. (2008). Pure alexia as a disconnection syndrome: new diffusion imaging evidence for an old concept. Cortex 44, 962–974. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein-Peterson, Z., Vasconcellos Faria, A., Mori, S., Hillis, A. E., and Tsapkini, K. (2012). Relatively normal repetition performance despite severe disruption of the left arcuate fasciculus. Neurocase 18, 521–526. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2011.633531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erika-Florence, M., Leech, R., and Hampshire, A. (2014). A functional network perspective on response inhibition and attentional control. Nat. Commun. 5:4073. doi: 10.1038/ncomms5073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farah, R., Dudley, J., Hutton, J., and Horowitz-Kraus, T. (2020). Maternal reading and fluency abilities are associated with diffusion properties of ventral and dorsal white matter tracts in their preschool-age children. Brain Cogn. 140:105532. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faulkner, J. W., and Wilshire, C. E. (2020). Mapping eloquent cortex: A voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study of core speech production capacities in brain tumour patients. Brain Lang. 200:104710. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2019.104710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Favre, E., Ballanger, B., Thobois, S., Broussolle, E., and Boulinguez, P. (2013). Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus, but not dopaminergic medication, improves proactive inhibitory control of movement initiation in PD’s disease. Neurotherapeutics 10, 154–167. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0166-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, L., Velásquez, C., García Porrero, J. A., de Lucas, E. M., and Martino, J. (2020). Heschl’s gyrus fiber intersection area: a new insight on the connectivity of the auditory-language hub. Neurosurg. Focus 48:E7. doi: 10.3171/2019.11.FOCUS19778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferraina, S., Brunamonti, E., Giusti, M. A., Costa, S., Genovesio, A., and Caminiti, R. (2009). Reaching in depth: hand position dominates over binocular eye position in the rostral superior parietal lobule. J. Neurosci. 29, 11461–11470. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1305-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forkel, S. J., Rogalski, E., Drossinos Sancho, N., D’Anna, L., Luque Laguna, P., Sridhar, J., et al. (2020). Anatomical evidence of an indirect pathway for word repetition. Neurology 94, e594–e606. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000008746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foville, A. L. (1844). Traité Complet De l’anatomie, De la Physiologie et de la Pathologie du Système Nerveux Cerebro-Spinal (Vol. 1). Fortin, Masson.

Frey, S., Campbell, J. S., Pike, G. B., and Petrides, M. (2008). Dissociating the human language pathways with high angular resolution diffusion fiber tractography. J. Neurosci. 28, 11435–11444. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2388-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fridriksson, J., den Ouden, D.-B., Hillis, A. E., Hickok, G., Rorden, C., Basilakos, A., et al. (2018). Anatomy of aphasia revisited. Brain 141, 848–862. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fridriksson, J., Guo, D., Fillmore, P., Holland, A., and Rorden, C. (2013). Damage to the anterior arcuate fasciculus predicts non-fluent speech production in aphasia. Brain 136, 3451–3460. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friederici, A. D. (2011). The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiol. Rev. 91, 1357–1392. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00006.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friederici, A. D. (2017). Neurobiology of syntax as the core of human language. Biolinguistics 11, 325–338. doi: 10.5964/bioling.9093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friederici, A. D., Bahlmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R. I., and Anwander, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: functional localization and structural connectivity. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U S A 103, 2458–2463. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509389103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friederici, A. D., Rüschemeyer, S.-A., Hahne, A., and Fiebach, C. J. (2003). The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cereb. Cortex 13, 170–177. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.2.170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujie, S., Namiki, C., Nishi, H., Yamada, M., Miyata, J., Sakata, D., et al. (2008). The role of the uncinate fasciculus in memory and emotional recognition in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 26, 432–439. doi: 10.1159/000165381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujii, M., Maesawa, S., Motomura, K., Futamura, M., Hayashi, Y., Koba, I., et al. (2015). Intraoperative subcortical mapping of a language-associated deep frontal tract connecting the superior frontal gyrus to Broca’s area in the dominant hemisphere of patients with glioma. J. Neurosurg. 122, 1390–1396. doi: 10.3171/2014.10.JNS14945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gajardo-Vidal, A., Lorca-Puls, D. L., team, P., Warner, H., Pshdary, B., Crinion, J. T., et al. (2021). Damage to Broca’s area does not contribute to long-term speech production outcome after stroke. Brain 144, 817–832. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galantucci, S., Tartaglia, M. C., Wilson, S. M., Henry, M. L., Filippi, M., Agosta, F., et al. (2011). White matter damage in primary progressive aphasias: a diffusion tensor tractography study. Brain 134, 3011–3029. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Guo, X., Liu, C., Mo, Y., and Wang, J. (2020). Right inferior frontal gyrus: an integrative hub in tonal bilinguals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 2152–2159. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giampiccolo, D., and Duffau, H. (2022). Controversy over the temporal cortical terminations of the left arcuate fasciculus: a reappraisal. Brain 145, 1242–1256. doi: 10.1093/brain/awac057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giampiccolo, D., Howells, H., Bährend, I., Schneider, H., Raffa, G., Rosenstock, T., et al. (2020). Preoperative transcranial magnetic stimulation for picture naming is reliable in mapping segments of the arcuate fasciculus. Brain Commun. 2:fcaa158. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcaa158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil-Robles, S., Carvallo, A., Jimenez, M. d. M., Gomez Caicoya, A., Martinez, R., Ruiz-Ocaña, C., et al. (2013). Double dissociation between visual recognition and picture naming: a study of the visual language connectivity using tractography and brain stimulation. Neurosurgery 72, 678–686. doi: 10.1227/NEU.0b013e318282a361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gleichgerrcht, E., Fridriksson, J., Rorden, C., and Bonilha, L. (2017). Connectome-based lesion-symptom mapping (CLSM): a novel approach to map neurological function. Neuroimage Clin. 16, 461–467. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldrick, M., and Rapp, B. (2007). Lexical and post-lexical phonological representations in spoken production. Cognition 102, 219–260. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez, M. R., Baaré, W. F. C., Hagler, D. J., Archibald, S., Vestergaard, M., and Madsen, K. S. (2021). Brain structure associations with phonemic and semantic fluency in typically-developing children. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 50:100982. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodglass, H. (1993). Understanding Aphasia. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Goodglass, H., Rosenberg, S., and Koplin, J. H. (1968). “Developments in applied psycholinguistic research,” in Studies in the Grammar of Aphasics, Eds. C. J. Rosenberg and J. H. Kopiin (New York: Macmillan), 177–208.

Google Scholar

Gottlieb, D. I., and Cowan, W. M. (1973). Autoradiographic studies of the commissural and ipsilateral association connection of the hippocampus and detentate gyrus of the rat. I. the commissural connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 149, 393–422. doi: 10.1002/cne.901490402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granek, J. A., Pisella, L., Blangero, A., Rossetti, Y., and Sergio, L. E. (2012). The role of the caudal superior parietal lobule in updating hand location in peripheral vision: further evidence from optic ataxia. PLoS One 7:e46619. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granger, S. J., Leal, S. L., Larson, M. S., Janecek, J. T., McMillan, L., Stern, H., et al. (2021). Integrity of the uncinate fasciculus is associated with emotional pattern separation-related fMRI signals in the hippocampal dentate and CA3. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 177:107359. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greicius, M. D., Supekar, K., Menon, V., and Dougherty, R. F. (2009). Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cereb. Cortex 19, 72–78. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffis, J. C., Nenert, R., Allendorfer, J. B., and Szaflarski, J. P. (2017). Damage to white matter bottlenecks contributes to language impairments after left hemispheric stroke. Neuroimage Clin. 14, 552–565. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.02.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, J. D., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Stamatakis, E. A., and Tyler, L. K. (2012). Functional organization of the neural language system: dorsal and ventral pathways are critical for syntax. Cereb. Cortex 23, 139–147. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grodzinsky, Y., and Santi, A. (2008). The battle for Broca’s region. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossi, D., Soricelli, A., Ponari, M., Salvatore, E., Quarantelli, M., Prinster, A., et al. (2014). Structural connectivity in a single case of progressive prosopagnosia: The role of the right inferior longitudinal fasciculus. Cortex 56, 111–120. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, M., Powers, J., Ash, S., McMillan, C., Burkholder, L., Irwin, D., et al. (2013). Disruption of large-scale neural networks in non-fluent/agrammatic variant primary progressive aphasia associated with frontotemporal degeneration pathology. Brain Lang. 127, 106–120. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grotheer, M., Yeatman, J., and Grill-Spector, K. (2021). White matter fascicles and cortical microstructure predict reading-related responses in human ventral temporal cortex. Neuroimage 227:117669. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guenther, F. H. (2016). Neural Control of Speech. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Guevara, M., Guevara, P., Román, C., and Mangin, J.-F. (2020). Superficial white matter: a review on the dMRI analysis methods and applications. Neuroimage 212:116673. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gullick, M. M., and Booth, J. R. (2014). Individual differences in crossmodal brain activity predict arcuate fasciculus connectivity in developing readers. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 1331–1346. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gullick, M. M., and Booth, J. R. (2015). The direct segment of the arcuate fasciculus is predictive of longitudinal reading change. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagoort, P. (2014). Nodes and networks in the neural architecture for language: broca’s region and beyond. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 28, 136–141. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halai, A. D., Woollams, A. M., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2017). Using principal component analysis to capture individual differences within a unified neuropsychological model of chronic post-stroke aphasia: revealing the unique neural correlates of speech fluency, phonology and semantics. Cortex 86, 275–289. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halwani, G., Loui, P., Rueber, T., and Schlaug, G. (2011). Effects of practice and experience on the arcuate fasciculus: comparing singers, instrumentalists and non-musicians [original research]. Front. Psychol. 2:156. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampshire, A. (2015). Putting the brakes on inhibitory models of frontal lobe function. Neuroimage 113, 340–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampshire, A., Chamberlain, S. R., Monti, M. M., Duncan, J., and Owen, A. M. (2010). The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control. Neuroimage 50, 1313–1319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, Z., Ma, Y., Gong, G., He, Y., Caramazza, A., and Bi, Y. (2013). White matter structural connectivity underlying semantic processing: evidence from brain damaged patients. Brain 136, 2952–2965. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, Z., Ma, Y., Gong, G., Huang, R., Song, L., and Bi, Y. (2016). White matter pathway supporting phonological encoding in speech production: a multi-modal imaging study of brain damage patients. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 577–589. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0926-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, D. Y., and Schnur, T. T. (2015). Distinct loci of lexical and semantic access deficits in aphasia: evidence from voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. Cortex 67, 37–58. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, D. Y., Wei, T., Ellmore, T. M., Hamilton, A. C., and Schnur, T. T. (2013). Neuropsychological evidence for the functional role of the uncinate fasciculus in semantic control. Neuropsychologia 51, 789–801. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.01.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasan, K. M., Iftikhar, A., Kamali, A., Kramer, L. A., Ashtari, M., Cirino, P. T., et al. (2009). Development and aging of the healthy human brain uncinate fasciculus across the lifespan using diffusion tensor tractography. Brain Res. 1276, 67–76. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hepner, C. R., and Nozari, N. (2019). Resource allocation in phonological working memory: same or different principles from vision? J. Mem. Lang. 106, 172–188. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2019.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hepner, C. R., and Nozari, N. (2020). The dual origin of lexical perseverations in aphasia: residual activation and incremental learning. Neuropsychologia 147:107603. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbet, G., Moritz-Gasser, S., Lemaitre, A.-L., Almairac, F., and Duffau, H. (2019). Functional compensation of the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus for picture naming. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 36, 140–157. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2018.1477749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbet, G., Yordanova, Y. N., and Duffau, H. (2017a). Left spatial neglect evoked by electrostimulation of the right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus. Brain Topogr. 30, 747–756. doi: 10.1007/s10548-017-0574-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbet, G., Moritz-Gasser, S., and Duffau, H. (2017b). Direct evidence for the contributive role of the right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus in non-verbal semantic cognition. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 1597–1610. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1294-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hervé, P. Y., Crivello, F., Perchey, G., Mazoyer, B., and Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2006). Handedness and cerebral anatomical asymmetries in young adult males. Neuroimage 29, 1066–1079. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G. (2012). Computational neuroanatomy of speech production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 135–145. doi: 10.1038/nrn3158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G., and Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition 92, 67–99. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G., and Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 393–402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Highley, J. R., Walker, M. A., Esiri, M. M., Crow, T. J., and Harrison, P. J. (2002). Asymmetry of the uncinate fasciculus: a post-mortem study of normal subjects and patients with schizophrenia. Cereb. Cortex 12, 1218–1224. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.11.1218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillis, A. E., Beh, Y. Y., Sebastian, R., Breining, B., Tippett, D. C., Wright, A., et al. (2018). Predicting recovery in acute poststroke aphasia. Ann. Neurol. 83, 612–622. doi: 10.1002/ana.25184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirshorn, E. A., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2006). Role of the left inferior frontal gyrus in covert word retrieval: Neural correlates of switching during verbal fluency. Neuropsychologia 44, 2547–2557. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hiyoshi-Taniguchi, K., Oishi, N., Namiki, C., Miyata, J., Murai, T., Cichocki, A., et al. (2015). The uncinate fasciculus as a predictor of conversion from amnestic mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer disease. J. Neuroimaging 25, 748–753. doi: 10.1111/jon.12196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodgetts, C. J., Postans, M., Shine, J. P., Jones, D. K., Lawrence, A. D., and Graham, K. S. (2015). Dissociable roles of the inferior longitudinal fasciculus and fornix in face and place perception. eLife 4:e07902. doi: 10.7554/eLife.07902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoeft, F., McCandliss, B. D., Black, J. M., Gantman, A., Zakerani, N., Hulme, C., et al. (2011). Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 361–366. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008950108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holl, N., Noblet, V., Rodrigo, S., Dietemann, J. L., Mekhbi, M. B., Kehrli, P., et al. (2011). Temporal lobe association fiber tractography as compared to histology and dissection. Surg. Radiol. Anat. 33, 713–722. doi: 10.1007/s00276-011-0816-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horel, J. A., and Misantone, L. J. (1976). Visual discrimination impaired by cutting temporal lobe connections. Science 193, 336–338. doi: 10.1126/science.819992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horowitz-Kraus, T., Wang, Y., Plante, E., and Holland, S. K. (2014). Involvement of the right hemisphere in reading comprehension: a DTI study. Brain Res. 1582, 34–44. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.05.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hua, K., Oishi, K., Zhang, J., Wakana, S., Yoshioka, T., Zhang, W., et al. (2008). Mapping of functional areas in the human cortex based on connectivity through association fibers. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1889–1895. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyde, C., Sciberras, E., Efron, D., Fuelscher, I., and Silk, T. (2021). Reduced fine motor competence in children with ADHD is associated with atypical microstructural organization within the superior longitudinal fasciculus. Brain Imaging Behav. 15, 727–737. doi: 10.1007/s11682-020-00280-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iaccarino, L., Crespi, C., Della Rosa, P. A., Catricalà, E., Guidi, L., Marcone, A., et al. (2015). The semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia: clinical and neuroimaging evidence in single subjects. PLoS One 10:e0120197. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ille, S., Engel, L., Kelm, A., Meyer, B., and Krieg, S. M. (2018). Language-eloquent white matter pathway tractography and the course of language function in glioma patients [Original Research]. Front. Oncol. 8:572. doi: 10.3389/fonc.2018.00572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Indefrey, P., and Levelt, W. J. (2004). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition 92, 101–144. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2002.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanova, M. V., Isaev, D. Y., Dragoy, O. V., Akinina, Y. S., Petrushevskiy, A. G., Fedina, O. N., et al. (2016). Diffusion-tensor imaging of major white matter tracts and their role in language processing in aphasia. Cortex 85, 165–181. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanova, M. V., Zhong, A., Turken, A., Baldo, J. V., and Dronkers, N. F. (2021). Functional contributions of the arcuate fasciculus to language processing [Original Research]. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:672665. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.672665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jahanshahi, M. (2013). Effects of deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus on inhibitory and executive control over prepotent responses in PD’s disease [Review]. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:118. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarret, J., Ferré, P., Chedid, G., Bedetti, C., Bore, A., Joanette, Y., et al. (2022). Functional network and structural connections involved in picture naming. Brain Lang. 231:105146. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2022.105146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, S., Kim, J.-H., Sung, G., Ko, Y.-G., Bang, M., Park, C.-I., et al. (2020). Uncinate fasciculus white matter connectivity related to impaired social perception and cross-sectional and longitudinal symptoms in patients with schizophrenia spectrum psychosis. Neurosci. Lett. 737:135144. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalyvas, A., Koutsarnakis, C., Komaitis, S., Karavasilis, E., Christidi, F., Skandalakis, G. P., et al. (2020). Mapping the human middle longitudinal fasciculus through a focused anatomo-imaging study: shifting the paradigm of its segmentation and connectivity pattern. Brain Struct. Funct. 225, 85–119. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01987-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamali, A., Flanders, A. E., Brody, J., Hunter, J. V., and Hasan, K. M. (2014). Tracing superior longitudinal fasciculus connectivity in the human brain using high resolution diffusion tensor tractography. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 269–281. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0498-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemerdere, R., de Champfleur, N. M., Deverdun, J., Cochereau, J., Moritz-Gasser, S., Herbet, G., et al. (2016). Role of the left frontal aslant tract in stuttering: a brain stimulation and tractographic study. J. Neurol. 263, 157–167. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7949-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemmerer, D. (2019). From blueprints to brain maps: the status of the Lemma Model in cognitive neuroscience. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 1085–1116. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2018.1537498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keser, Z., Hillis, A. E., Schulz, P. E., Hasan, K. M., and Nelson, F. M. (2020). Frontal aslant tracts as correlates of lexical retrieval in MS. Neurol Res. 42, 805–810. doi: 10.1080/01616412.2020.1781454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kier, E. L., Staib, L. H., Davis, L. M., and Bronen, R. A. (2004). MR imaging of the temporal stem: anatomic dissection tractography of the uncinate fasciculus, inferior occipitofrontal fasciculus and meyer’s loop of the optic radiation. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 25, 677–691. Available online at: http://www.ajnr.org/content/ajnr/25/5/677.full.tif.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kim, S. H., and Jang, S. H. (2013). Prediction of aphasia outcome using diffusion tensor tractography for arcuate fasciculus in stroke. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 34, 785–790. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinoshita, M., de Champfleur, N. M., Deverdun, J., Moritz-Gasser, S., Herbet, G., and Duffau, H. (2015). Role of fronto-striatal tract and frontal aslant tract in movement and speech: an axonal mapping study. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 3399–3412. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0863-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitis, O., Ozalay, O., Zengin, E. B., Haznedaroglu, D., Eker, M. C., Yalvac, D., et al. (2012). Reduced left uncinate fasciculus fractional anisotropy in deficit schizophrenia but not in non-deficit schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 66, 34–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2011.02293.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kljajevic, V., Dyrba, M., Kasper, E., and Teipel, S. (2016). Is the left uncinate fasciculus associated with verbal fluency decline in mild Alzheimer’s disease? Transl. Neurosci. 7, 89–91. doi: 10.1515/tnsci-2016-0014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kljajevic, V., and Erramuzpe, A. (2019). Dorsal white matter integrity and name retrieval in midlife. Curr. Aging Sci. 12, 55–61. doi: 10.2174/1874609812666190614110214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kourtidou, E., Kasselimis, D., Angelopoulou, G., Karavasilis, E., Velonakis, G., Kelekis, N., et al. (2021). The role of the right hemisphere white matter tracts in chronic aphasic patients after damage of the language tracts in the left hemisphere. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:635750. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.635750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kronfeld-Duenias, V., Amir, O., Ezrati-Vinacour, R., Civier, O., and Ben-Shachar, M. (2016). The frontal aslant tract underlies speech fluency in persistent developmental stuttering. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 365–381. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0912-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubicki, M., Park, H., Westin, C. F., Nestor, P. G., Mulkern, R. V., Maier, S. E., et al. (2005). DTI and MTR abnormalities in schizophrenia: analysis of white matter integrity. Neuroimage 26, 1109–1118. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubicki, M., Westin, C. F., Maier, S. E., Frumin, M., Nestor, P. G., Salisbury, D. F., et al. (2002). Uncinate fasciculus findings in schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance diffusion tensor imaging study. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 813–820. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.5.813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, U., and Padakannaya, P. (2019). The effect of written scripts’ dissimilarity over ventral and dorsal reading pathway: combined fMRI and DTI study. Read. Writ. 32, 2311–2325. doi: 10.1007/S11145-019-09952-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kümmerer, D., Hartwigsen, G., Kellmeyer, P., Glauche, V., Mader, I., Klöppel, S., et al. (2013). Damage to ventral and dorsal language pathways in acute aphasia. Brain 136, 619–629. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kyeong, S., Kang, H., Kyeong, S., and Kim, D. H. (2019). Differences in brain areas affecting language function after stroke. Stroke 50, 2956–2959. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.119.026222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latini, F., Mårtensson, J., Larsson, E.-M., Fredrikson, M., Åhs, F., Hjortberg, M., et al. (2017). Segmentation of the inferior longitudinal fasciculus in the human brain: a white matter dissection and diffusion tensor tractography study. Brain Res. 1675, 102–115. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauro, L. J. R., Miracapillo, C., Casarotti, A., Bello, L., and Papagno, C. (2010). What is the role of the uncinate fasciculus? Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 6, 84–85. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.08.043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawes, I. N. C., Barrick, T. R., Murugam, V., Spierings, N., Evans, D. R., Song, M., et al. (2008). Atlas-based segmentation of white matter tracts of the human brain using diffusion tensor tractography and comparison with classical dissection. Neuroimage 39, 62–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J. F., Poupon, C., Clark, C. A., Pappata, S., Molko, N., et al. (2001). Diffusion tensor imaging: concepts and applications. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 13, 534–546. doi: 10.1002/jmri.1076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebel, C., and Beaulieu, C. (2009). Lateralization of the arcuate fasciculus from childhood to adulthood and its relation to cognitive abilities in children. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3563–3573. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leclercq, D., Duffau, H., Delmaire, C., Capelle, L., Gatignol, P., Ducros, M., et al. (2010). Comparison of diffusion tensor imaging tractography of language tracts and intraoperative subcortical stimulations. J. Neurosurg. 112, 503–511. doi: 10.3171/2009.8.JNS09558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemaire, J. J., Golby, A., Wells, W. M., 3rd, Pujol, S., Tie, Y., Rigolo, L., et al. (2013). Extended Broca’s area in the functional connectome of language in adults: combined cortical and subcortical single-subject analysis using fMRI and DTI tractography. Brain Topogr. 26, 428–441. doi: 10.1007/s10548-012-0257-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., and Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 1–38. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99001776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Zatorre, R. J., and Du, Y. (2021). The microstructural plasticity of the arcuate fasciculus undergirds improved speech in noise perception in musicians. Cereb. Cortex 31, 3975–3985. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, M., Zhang, Y., Song, L., Huang, R., Ding, J., Fang, Y., et al. (2017). Structural connectivity subserving verbal fluency revealed by lesion-behavior mapping in stroke patients. Neuropsychologia 101, 85–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liégeois, F. J., Mahony, K., Connelly, A., Pigdon, L., Tournier, J.-D., and Morgan, A. T. (2013). Pediatric traumatic brain injury: language outcomes and their relationship to the arcuate fasciculus. Brain Lang. 127, 388–398. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linortner, P., McDaniel, C., Shahid, M., Levine, T. F., Tian, L., Cholerton, B., et al. (2020). White matter hyperintensities related to PD’s disease executive function. Mov. Disord. Clin. Pract. 7, 629–638. doi: 10.1002/mdc3.12956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo, Y.-C., Soong, W.-T., Gau, S. S.-F., Wu, Y.-Y., Lai, M.-C., Yeh, F.-C., et al. (2011). The loss of asymmetry and reduced interhemispheric connectivity in adolescents with autism: a study using diffusion spectrum imaging tractography. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 192, 60–66. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Barroso, D., Catani, M., Ripollés, P., Dell’Acqua, F., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., and de Diego-Balaguer, R. (2013). Word learning is mediated by the left arcuate fasciculus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 110, 13168–13173. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1301696110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez-Barroso, D., de Diego-Balaguer, R., Cunillera, T., Camara, E., Münte, T. F., and Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2011). Language learning under working memory constraints correlates with microstructural differences in the ventral language pathway. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2742–2750. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lüders, H., Lesser, R. P., Dinner, D. S., Morris, H. H., Wyllie, E., and Godoy, J. (1988). Localization of cortical function: new information from extraoperative monitoring of patients with epilepsy. Epilepsia 29, S56–S65. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1988.tb05799.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, C., Makaretz, S., Stepanovic, M., Papadimitriou, G., Quimby, M., Palanivelu, S., et al. (2020). Middle longitudinal fascicle is associated with semantic processing deficits in primary progressive aphasia. Neuroimage Clin. 25:102115. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.102115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madden, D. J., Bennett, I. J., Burzynska, A., Potter, G. G., Chen, N.-k., and Song, A. W. (2012). Diffusion tensor imaging of cerebral white matter integrity in cognitive aging. Biochim. Biophys. Acta 1822, 386–400. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madhavan, K. M., McQueeny, T., Howe, S. R., Shear, P., and Szaflarski, J. (2014). Superior longitudinal fasciculus and language functioning in healthy aging. Brain Res. 1562, 11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusdottir, S., Fillmore, P., den Ouden, D. B., Hjaltason, H., Rorden, C., Kjartansson, O., et al. (2013). Damage to left anterior temporal cortex predicts impairment of complex syntactic processing: a lesion-symptom mapping study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 2715–2723. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makris, N., and Pandya, D. N. (2008). The extreme capsule in humans and rethinking of the language circuitry. Brain Struct. Funct. 213, 343–358. doi: 10.1007/s00429-008-0199-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makris, N., Preti, M. G., Wassermann, D., Rathi, Y., Papadimitriou, G. M., Yergatian, C., et al. (2013). Human middle longitudinal fascicle: segregation and behavioral-clinical implications of two distinct fiber connections linking temporal pole and superior temporal gyrus with the angular gyrus or superior parietal lobule using multi-tensor tractography. Brain Imaging Behav. 7, 335–352. doi: 10.1007/s11682-013-9235-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makris, N., Zhu, A., Papadimitriou, G. M., Mouradian, P., Ng, I., Scaccianoce, E., et al. (2017). Mapping temporo-parietal and temporo-occipital cortico-cortical connections of the human middle longitudinal fascicle in subject-specific, probabilistic and stereotaxic Talairach spaces. Brain Imaging Behav. 11, 1258–1277. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9589-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maldonado, I. L., Moritz-Gasser, S., and Duffau, H. (2011). Does the left superior longitudinal fascicle subserve language semantics? A brain electrostimulation study. Brain Struct. Funct. 216, 263–274. doi: 10.1007/s00429-011-0309-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandelli, M. L., Caverzasi, E., Binney, R. J., Henry, M. L., Lobach, I., Block, N., et al. (2014). Frontal white matter tracts sustaining speech production in primary progressive aphasia. J. Neurosci. 34, 9754–9767. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3464-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandonnet, E., Dadoun, Y., Poisson, I., Madadaki, C., Froelich, S., and Lozeron, P. (2016). Axono-cortical evoked potentials: a proof-of-concept study. Neurochirurgie 62, 67–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuchi.2015.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandonnet, E., Gatignol, P., and Duffau, H. (2009). Evidence for an occipito-temporal tract underlying visual recognition in picture naming. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 111, 601–605. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2009.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandonnet, E., Nouet, A., Gatignol, P., Capelle, L., and Duffau, H. (2007). Does the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus play a role in language? a brain stimulation study. Brain 130, 623–629. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandonnet, E., Winkler, P. A., and Duffau, H. (2010). Direct electrical stimulation as an input gate into brain functional networks: principles, advantages and limitations. Acta Neurochir. (Wien) 152, 185–193. doi: 10.1007/s00701-009-0469-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchina, S., Zhu, L. L., Norton, A., Zipse, L., Wan, C. Y., and Schlaug, G. (2011). Impairment of speech production predicted by lesion load of the left arcuate fasciculus. Stroke 42, 2251–2256. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.606103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcotte, K., Graham, N. L., Fraser, K. C., Meltzer, J. A., Tang-Wai, D. F., Chow, T. W., et al. (2017). White matter disruption and connected speech in non-fluent and semantic variants of primary progressive aphasia. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. Extra 7, 52–73. doi: 10.1159/000456710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mars, R. B., Foxley, S., Verhagen, L., Jbabdi, S., Sallet, J., Noonan, M. P., et al. (2016). The extreme capsule fiber complex in humans and macaque monkeys: a comparative diffusion MRI tractography study. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 4059–4071. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1146-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mars, R. B., Piekema, C., Coles, M. G. H., Hulstijn, W., and Toni, I. (2007). On the programming and reprogramming of actions. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2972–2979. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin-Chang, S. L., and Levy, B. A. (2006). Word reading fluency: a transfer appropriate processing accountof fluency transfer. Read. Writ. 19, 517–542. doi: 10.1007/s11145-006-9007-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinez Oeckel, A., Rijntjes, M., Glauche, V., Kümmerer, D., Kaller, C. P., Egger, K., et al. (2021). The extreme capsule and aphasia: proof-of-concept of a new way relating structure to neurological symptoms. Brain Commun. 3:fcab040. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcab040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martino, J., Brogna, C., Robles, S. G., Vergani, F., and Duffau, H. (2010). Anatomic dissection of the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus revisited in the lights of brain stimulation data. Cortex 46, 691–699. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martino, J., and De Lucas, E. M. (2014). Subcortical anatomy of the lateral association fascicles of the brain: a review. Clin. Anat. 27, 563–569. doi: 10.1002/ca.22321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martino, J., De Witt Hamer, P. C., Berger, M. S., Lawton, M. T., Arnold, C. M., de Lucas, E. M., et al. (2013). Analysis of the subcomponents and cortical terminations of the perisylvian superior longitudinal fasciculus: a fiber dissection and DTI tractography study. Brain Struct. Funct. 218, 105–121. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0386-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matchin, W., Hammerly, C., and Lau, E. (2017). The role of the IFG and pSTS in syntactic prediction: evidence from a parametric study of hierarchical structure in fMRI. Cortex 88, 106–123. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matchin, W., and Hickok, G. (2020). The cortical organization of syntax. Cerebral Cortex 30, 1481–1498. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClelland, J. L., and Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cogn. Psychol. 18, 1–86. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(86)90015-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonagh, D., Fisher, A., and Nozari, N. (2020). “Do taxonomic and associative relations affect word production in the same way?,” in Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Eds. S. Denison, M. Mack, Y. Xu and B. C. Armstrong (Cognitive Science Society), 544–550.

Google Scholar

McKinnon, E. T., Fridriksson, J., Basilakos, A., Hickok, G., Hillis, A. E., Spampinato, M. V., et al. (2018). Types of naming errors in chronic post-stroke aphasia are dissociated by dual stream axonal loss. Sci. Rep. 8:14352. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-32457-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mechelli, A., Price, C. J., Friston, K. J., and Ashburner, J. (2005). Voxel-based morphometry of the human brain: methods and applications. Curr. Med. Imaging Rev. 1, 105–113. doi: 10.2174/1573405054038726

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menjot de Champfleur, N., Lima Maldonado, I., Moritz-Gasser, S., Machi, P., Le Bars, E., Bonafé, A., et al. (2013). Middle longitudinal fasciculus delineation within language pathways: a diffusion tensor imaging study in human. Eur. J. Radiol. 82, 151–157. doi: 10.1016/j.ejrad.2012.05.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesulam, M.-M., Thompson, C. K., Weintraub, S., and Rogalski, E. J. (2015). The Wernicke conundrum and the anatomy of language comprehension in primary progressive aphasia. Brain 138, 2423–2437. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mettler, F. A. (1935). Corticifugal fiber connections of the cortex of Macaca mulatta. The temporal region. J. Comp. Neurol. 63, 25–47. doi: 10.1002/cne.900630104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, A. S., and Bock, K. (1992). The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: blocking or partial activation? Mem. Cognit. 20, 715–726. doi: 10.3758/bf03202721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meynert, T. (1885). A Clinical Treatise on Disease of the Fore-Brain, based upon a Study of its Structure, Functions and Nutrition. Translated by Bernard. Sachs. New York and London: G. P. Putnam’s Sons.

Google Scholar

Middleton, F. A., and Strick, P. L. (2000). Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits. Brain Res. Rev. 31, 236–250. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00040-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikuni, N., Ohara, S., Ikeda, A., Hayashi, N., Nishida, N., Taki, J., et al. (2006). Evidence for a wide distribution of negative motor areas in the perirolandic cortex. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 33–40. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.08.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, H. E., and Guenther, F. H. (2021). Modelling speech motor programming and apraxia of speech in the DIVA/GODIVA neurocomputational framework. Aphasiology 35, 424–441. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2020.1765307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mills, B. D., Lai, J., Brown, T. T., Erhart, M., Halgren, E., Reilly, J., et al. (2013). White matter microstructure correlates of narrative production in typically developing children and children with high functioning autism. Neuropsychologia 51, 1933–1941. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirman, D., Zhang, Y., Wang, Z., Coslett, H. B., and Schwartz, M. F. (2015). The ins and outs of meaning: behavioral and neuroanatomical dissociation of semantically-driven word retrieval and multimodal semantic recognition in aphasia. Neuropsychologia 76, 208–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, E., Schaefer, R. S., Bastin, M. E., Roberts, N., and Overy, K. (2017). Diffusion tensor MRI tractography reveals increased fractional anisotropy (FA) in arcuate fasciculus following music-cued motor training. Brain Cogn. 116, 40–46. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mori, S., and Zhang, J. (2006). Principles of diffusion tensor imaging and its applications to basic neuroscience research. Neuron 51, 527–539. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morikawa, M., Kiuchi, K., Taoka, T., Nagauchi, K., Kichikawa, K., and Kishimoto, T. (2010). Uncinate fasciculus-correlated cognition in Alzheimer’s disease: a diffusion tensor imaging study by tractography. Psychogeriatrics 10, 15–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8301.2010.00312.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz-Gasser, S., Herbet, G., and Duffau, H. (2013). Mapping the connectivity underlying multimodal (verbal and non-verbal) semantic processing: a brain electrostimulation study. Neuropsychologia 51, 1814–1822. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motomura, K., Chalise, L., Ohka, F., Aoki, K., Tanahashi, K., Hirano, M., et al. (2018). Supratotal resection of diffuse frontal lower grade gliomas with awake brain mapping, preserving motor, language and neurocognitive functions. World Neurosurg. 119, 30–39. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2018.07.193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motomura, K., Fujii, M., Maesawa, S., Kuramitsu, S., Natsume, A., and Wakabayashi, T. (2014). Association of dorsal inferior frontooccipital fasciculus fibers in the deep parietal lobe with both reading and writing processes: a brain mapping study: case report. J. Neurosurg. 121, 142–148. doi: 10.3171/2014.2.JNS131234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mukherjee, P., Chung, S. W., Berman, J. I., Hess, C. P., and Henry, R. G. (2008). Diffusion tensor MR imaging and fiber tractography: technical considerations. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 29, 843–852. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A1052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Multani, N., Galantucci, S., Wilson, S. M., Shany-Ur, T., Poorzand, P., Growdon, M. E., et al. (2017). Emotion detection deficits and changes in personality traits linked to loss of white matter integrity in primary progressive aphasia. Neuroimage Clin. 16, 447–454. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nachev, P., Kennard, C., and Husain, M. (2008). Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 856–869. doi: 10.1038/nrn2478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nachev, P., Wydell, H., O’Neill, K., Husain, M., and Kennard, C. (2007). The role of the pre-supplementary motor area in the control of action. Neuroimage 36, T155–T163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagae, L. M., Zarnow, D. M., Blaskey, L., Dell, J., Khan, S. Y., Qasmieh, S., et al. (2012). Elevated mean diffusivity in the left hemisphere superior longitudinal fasciculus in autism spectrum disorders increases with more profound language impairment. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 33, 1720–1725. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naito, E., Scheperjans, F., Eickhoff, S. B., Amunts, K., Roland, P. E., Zilles, K., et al. (2008). Human superior parietal lobule is involved in somatic perception of bimanual interaction with an external object. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 695–703. doi: 10.1152/jn.00529.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakajima, R., Yordanova, Y. N., Duffau, H., and Herbet, G. (2018). Neuropsychological evidence for the crucial role of the right arcuate fasciculus in the face-based mentalizing network: a disconnection analysis. Neuropsychologia 115, 179–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neef, N. E., Anwander, A., Bütfering, C., Schmidt-Samoa, C., Friederici, A. D., Paulus, W., et al. (2018). Structural connectivity of right frontal hyperactive areas scales with stuttering severity. Brain 141, 191–204. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nomura, K., Kazui, H., Tokunaga, H., Hirata, M., Goto, T., Goto, Y., et al. (2013). Possible roles of the dominant uncinate fasciculus in naming objects: a case report of intraoperative electrical stimulation on a patient with a brain tumour. Behav. Neurol. 27, 229–234. doi: 10.3233/BEN-110249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Northam, G. B., Liégeois, F., Tournier, J.-D., Croft, L. J., Johns, P. N., Chong, W. K., et al. (2012). Interhemispheric temporal lobe connectivity predicts language impairment in adolescents born preterm. Brain 135, 3781–3798. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N. (2019). The dual origin of semantic errors in access deficit: activation vs. inhibition deficit. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 36, 31–53. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2019.1587397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N. (2020). A comprehension- or a production-based monitor? Response to roelofs (2020). J. Cogn. 3:19. doi: 10.5334/joc.102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N. (2021). “Neural basis of word production,” in The Oxford Handbook of The Mental Lexicon, Eds. L. R. Gleitman, A. Papafragou and J. C. Trueswell (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 552–574.

Google Scholar

Nozari, N., Arnold, J. E., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014a). The effects of anodal stimulation of the left prefrontal cortex on sentence production. Brain Stimul. 7, 784–792. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.07.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., Woodard, K., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014b). Consequences of cathodal stimulation for behavior: when does it help and when does it hurt performance? PLoS One 9:e84338. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., and Dell, G. S. (2013). How damaged brains repeat words: a computational approach. Brain Lang. 126, 327–337. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., Dell, G. S., and Schwartz, M. F. (2011). Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production. Cogn. Psychol. 63, 1–33. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., and Faroqi-Shah, Y. (2017). Investigating the origin of nonfluency in aphasia: a path modeling approach to neuropsychology. Cortex 95, 119–135. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., and Hepner, C. (2019a). To select or to wait? Response to the commentaries. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 36, 226–233. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2019.1632280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., and Hepner, C. (2019b). To select or to wait? The importance of criterion setting in debates of competitive lexical selection. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 36, 193–207. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2018.1476335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., Kittredge, A. K., Dell, G. S., and Schwartz, M. F. (2010). Naming and repetition in aphasia: steps, routes and frequency effects. J. Mem. Lang. 63, 541–559. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., Mirman, D., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2016a). The ventrolateral prefrontal cortex facilitates processing of sentential context to locate referents. Brain Lang. 157–158, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., Freund, M., Breining, B., Rapp, B., and Gordon, B. (2016b). Cognitive control during selection and repair in word production. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 31, 886–903. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1157194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., and Novick, J. (2017). Monitoring and control in language production. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26, 403–410. doi: 10.1177/0963721417702419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., and Omaki, A. (2022). An investigation of the dependency of subject-verb agreement on inhibitory control processes in sentence production. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/9pcmg

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., and Pinet, S. (2020). A critical review of the behavioral, neuroimaging and electrophysiological studies of co-activation of representations during word production. J. Neurolinguist. 53:100875. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2019.100875

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2013). More attention when speaking: does it help or does it hurt? Evidence from tDCS. Neuropsychologia 51, 2770–2780. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozari, N., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2016). “Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in processing of words and sentences,” in Neurobiology of Language, Eds. G. Hickok and S. L. Small (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 569–584.

Google Scholar

Nugiel, T., Alm, K. H., and Olson, I. R. (2016). Individual differences in white matter microstructure predict semantic control. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 1003–1016. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0448-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obeso, I., Wilkinson, L., Casabona, E., Speekenbrink, M., Luisa Bringas, M., Álvarez, M., et al. (2014). The subthalamic nucleus and inhibitory control: impact of subthalamotomy in PD’s disease. Brain 137, 1470–1480. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oechslin, M., Imfeld, A., Loenneker, T., Meyer, M., and Jäncke, L. (2010). The plasticity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus as a function of musical expertise: a diffusion tensor imaging study [Original Research]. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 3:76. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.076.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oishi, K., Faria, A. V., Hsu, J., Tippett, D., Mori, S., and Hillis, A. E. (2015). Critical role of the right uncinate fasciculus in emotional empathy. Ann. Neurol. 77, 68–74. doi: 10.1002/ana.24300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oishi, K., Faria, A. V., Van Zijl, P. C., and Mori, S. (2010). MRI Atlas of Human White Matter. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Oishi, K., Mori, S., Donohue, P. K., Ernst, T., Anderson, L., Buchthal, S., et al. (2011). Multi-contrast human neonatal brain atlas: application to normal neonate development analysis. Neuroimage 56, 8–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oishi, K., Zilles, K., Amunts, K., Faria, A., Jiang, H., Li, X., et al. (2008). Human brain white matter atlas: identification and assignment of common anatomical structures in superficial white matter. Neuroimage 43, 447–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortibus, E., Verhoeven, J., Sunaert, S., Casteels, I., de Cock, P., and Lagae, L. (2012). Integrity of the inferior longitudinal fasciculus and impaired object recognition in children: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 54, 38–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04147.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panesar, S. S., Yeh, F.-C., Jacquesson, T., Hula, W., and Fernandez-Miranda, J. C. (2018). A quantitative tractography study into the connectivity, segmentation and laterality of the human inferior longitudinal fasciculus. Front. Neuroanat. 12:47. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2018.00047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papagno, C., Casarotti, A., Comi, A., Pisoni, A., Lucchelli, F., Bizzi, A., et al. (2016). Long-term proper name anomia after removal of the uncinate fasciculus. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 687–694. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0920-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papagno, C., Miracapillo, C., Casarotti, A., Romero Lauro, L. J., Castellano, A., Falini, A., et al. (2011). What is the role of the uncinate fasciculus? Surgical removal and proper name retrieval. Brain 134, 405–414. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papinutto, N., Galantucci, S., Mandelli, M. L., Gesierich, B., Jovicich, J., Caverzasi, E., et al. (2016). Structural connectivity of the human anterior temporal lobe: a diffusion magnetic resonance imaging study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 2210–2222. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papoutsi, M., Stamatakis, E. A., Griffiths, J., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., and Tyler, L. K. (2011). Is left fronto-temporal connectivity essential for syntax? Effective connectivity, tractography and performance in left-hemisphere damaged patients. Neuroimage 58, 656–664. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, H.-J., Westin, C.-F., Kubicki, M., Maier, S. E., Niznikiewicz, M., Baer, A., et al. (2004). White matter hemisphere asymmetries in healthy subjects and in schizophrenia: a diffusion tensor MRI study. Neuroimage 23, 213–223. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passamonti, L., Fairchild, G., Fornito, A., Goodyer, I. M., Nimmo-Smith, I., Hagan, C. C., et al. (2012). Abnormal anatomical connectivity between the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in conduct disorder. PLoS One 7:e48789. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, K., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2016). “The Hub-and-Spoke Hypothesis of Semantic Memory, ” in Neurobiology of Language, Vol. 102, Eds. G. Hickok and S. L. Small (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 765–775. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00061-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, D. K., Van Petten, C., Beeson, P. M., Rapcsak, S. Z., and Plante, E. (2014). Bidirectional iterative parcellation of diffusion weighted imaging data: separating cortical regions connected by the arcuate fasciculus and extreme capsule. Neuroimage 102, 704–716. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peelle, J. E., Troiani, V., Gee, J., Moore, P., McMillan, C., Vesely, L., et al. (2008). Sentence comprehension and voxel-based morphometry in progressive nonfluent aphasia, semantic dementia and nonaphasic frontotemporal dementia. J. Neurolinguistics 21, 418–432. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2008.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perron, M., Theaud, G., Descoteaux, M., and Tremblay, P. (2021). The frontotemporal organization of the arcuate fasciculus and its relationship with speech perception in young and older amateur singers and non-singers. Hum. Brain Mapp. 42, 3058–3076. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, B. D., Szeszko, P. R., Radua, J., Ikuta, T., Gruner, P., DeRosse, P., et al. (2012). White matter development in adolescence: diffusion tensor imaging and meta-analytic results. Schizophr. Bull. 38, 1308–1317. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, M., and Pandya, D. N. (1984). Projections to the frontal cortex from the posterior parietal region in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 228, 105–116. doi: 10.1002/cne.902280110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, M., and Pandya, D. N. (2002). Comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis of the human and the macaque ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and corticocortical connection patterns in the monkey. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16, 291–310. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2001.02090.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, M., and Pandya, D. N. (2006). Efferent association pathways originating in the caudal prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 498, 227–251. doi: 10.1002/cne.21048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phan, K. L., Orlichenko, A., Boyd, E., Angstadt, M., Coccaro, E. F., Liberzon, I., et al. (2009). Preliminary evidence of white matter abnormality in the uncinate fasciculus in generalized social anxiety disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 691–694. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, O. R., Clark, K. A., Woods, R. P., Subotnik, K. L., Asarnow, R. F., Nuechterlein, K. H., et al. (2011). Topographical relationships between arcuate fasciculus connectivity and cortical thickness. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1788–1801. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pillay, S. B., Binder, J. R., Humphries, C., Gross, W. L., and Book, D. S. (2017). Lesion localization of speech comprehension deficits in chronic aphasia. Neurology 88, 970–975. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinet, S., and Nozari, N. (2018). “Twisting fingers”: the case for interactivity in typed language production. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 1449–1457. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1452-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinet, S., and Nozari, N. (2023). Different electrophysiological signatures of similarity-induced and stroop-like interference in language production. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/zbfp3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poustka, L., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Henze, R., Vomstein, K., Haffner, J., and Sieltjes, B. (2012). Fronto-temporal disconnectivity and symptom severity in children with autism spectrum disorder. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 13, 269–280. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2011.591824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, J. P., McMillan, C. T., Brun, C. C., Yushkevich, P. A., Zhang, H., Gee, J. C., et al. (2013). White matter disease correlates with lexical retrieval deficits in primary progressive aphasia. Front. Neurol. 4:212. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, C. J. (2010). The anatomy of language: a review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1191, 62–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05444.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, C. J. (2012). A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. Neuroimage 62, 816–847. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, A. R., Bonner, M. F., Peelle, J. E., and Grossman, M. (2015). Converging evidence for the neuroanatomic basis of combinatorial semantics in the angular gyrus. J. Neurosci. 35, 3276–3284. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3446-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Primaßin, A., Scholtes, N., Heim, S., Huber, W., Neuschäfer, M., Binkofski, F., et al. (2015). Determinants of concurrent motor and language recovery during intensive therapy in chronic stroke patients: four single-case studies. Front. Neurol. 6:215. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pugliese, L., Catani, M., Ameis, S., Dell’Acqua, F., de Schotten, M. T., Murphy, C., et al. (2009). The anatomy of extended limbic pathways in Asperger syndrome: a preliminary diffusion tensor imaging tractography study. Neuroimage 47, 427–434. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pustina, D., Doucet, G., Evans, J., Sharan, A., Sperling, M., Skidmore, C., et al. (2014). Distinct types of white matter changes are observed after anterior temporal lobectomy in epilepsy. PLoS One 9:e104211. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0104211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raffa, G., Bährend, I., Schneider, H., Faust, K., Germanò, A., Vajkoczy, P., et al. (2016). A novel technique for region and linguistic specific nTMS-based DTI fiber tracking of language pathways in brain tumor patients. Front. Neurosci. 10:552. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramanan, S., Piguet, O., and Irish, M. (2018). Rethinking the role of the angular gyrus in remembering the past and imagining the future: the contextual integration model. Neuroscientist 24, 342–352. doi: 10.1177/1073858417735514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramsey, L. E., Siegel, J. S., Lang, C. E., Strube, M., Shulman, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2017). Behavioural clusters and predictors of performance during recovery from stroke. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0038. doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapp, B., and Fischer-Baum, S. (2014). “Representation of orthographic knowledge,” in Oxford Handbook of Language Production, Eds. M. A. Goldrick, V. Ferreria, and M. Miozzo (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 338–357. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199735471.013.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapp, B., and Goldrick, M. (2000). Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. Psychol. Rev. 107, 460–499. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.3.460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riès, S. K., Dhillon, R. K., Clarke, A., King-Stephens, D., Laxer, K. D., Weber, P. B., et al. (2017). Spatiotemporal dynamics of word retrieval in speech production revealed by cortical high-frequency band activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 114, E4530–E4538. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620669114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rilling, J. K., Glasser, M. F., Preuss, T. M., Ma, X., Zhao, T., Hu, X., et al. (2008). The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 426–428. doi: 10.1038/nn2072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizio, A. A., and Diaz, M. T. (2016). Language, aging and cognition: frontal aslant tract and superior longitudinal fasciculus contribute toward working memory performance in older adults. Neuroreport 27, 689–693. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, G., Shallice, T., Bozzali, M., and Cipolotti, L. (2010). Conceptual proposition selection and the LIFG: neuropsychological evidence from a focal frontal group. Neuropsychologia 48, 1652–1663. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodrigo, S., Oppenheim, C., Chassoux, F., Golestani, N., Cointepas, Y., Poupon, C., et al. (2007). Uncinate fasciculus fiber tracking in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Initial findings. Eur. Radiol. 17, 1663–1668. doi: 10.1007/s00330-006-0558-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Aranda, C., Waterloo, K., Johnsen, S. H., Eldevik, P., Sparr, S., Wikran, G. C., et al. (2016). Neuroanatomical correlates of verbal fluency in early Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging. Brain Lang. 155–156, 24–35. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogalsky, C., and Hickok, G. (2011). The role of broca’s area in sentence comprehension. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1664–1680. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogalsky, C., LaCroix, A. N., Chen, K.-H., Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Love, T., et al. (2018). The neurobiology of agrammatic sentence comprehension: a lesion study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 234–255. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolheiser, T., Stamatakis, E. A., and Tyler, L. K. (2011). Dynamic processing in the human language system: synergy between the arcuate fascicle and extreme capsule. J. Neurosci. 31, 16949–16957. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2725-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, E., Cheng, H., Kroll, J. F., Diaz, M. T., and Newman, S. D. (2017). Changes in white-matter connectivity in late second language learners: evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Front. Psychol. 8:2040. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rushworth, M. F. S., Behrens, T. E. J., Rudebeck, P. H., and Walton, M. E. (2007). Contrasting roles for cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex in decisions and social behaviour. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 168–176. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sali, G., Briggs, R. G., Conner, A. K., Rahimi, M., Baker, C. M., Burks, J. D., et al. (2018). A connectomic atlas of the human cerebrum—chapter 11: tractographic description of the inferior longitudinal fasciculus. Oper. Neurosurg. (Hagerstown) 15, S423–S428. doi: 10.1093/ons/opy265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samuel, A. G. (2020). Psycholinguists should resist the allure of linguistic units as perceptual units. J. Mem. Lang. 111:104070. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2019.104070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanvito, F., Caverzasi, E., Riva, M., Jordan, K. M., Blasi, V., Scifo, P., et al. (2020). fMRI-targeted high-angular resolution diffusion mr tractography to identify functional language tracts in healthy controls and glioma patients. Front. Neurosci. 14:225. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarkar, S., Craig, M. C., Catani, M., Dell’Acqua, F., Fahy, T., Deeley, Q., et al. (2013). Frontotemporal white-matter microstructural abnormalities in adolescents with conduct disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychol. Med. 43, 401–411. doi: 10.1017/S003329171200116X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarubbo, S., De Benedictis, A., Maldonado, I. L., Basso, G., and Duffau, H. (2013). Frontal terminations for the inferior fronto-occipital fascicle: anatomical dissection, DTI study and functional considerations on a multi-component bundle. Brain Struct. Funct. 218, 21–37. doi: 10.1007/s00429-011-0372-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarubbo, S., De Benedictis, A., Merler, S., Mandonnet, E., Balbi, S., Granieri, E., et al. (2015). Towards a functional atlas of human white matter. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 3117–3136. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saur, D., Kreher, B. W., Schnell, S., Kümmerer, D., Kellmeyer, P., Vry, M.-S., et al. (2008). Ventral and dorsal pathways for language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 105, 18035–18040. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805234105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saur, D., Schelter, B., Schnell, S., Kratochvil, D., Küpper, H., Kellmeyer, P., et al. (2010). Combining functional and anatomical connectivity reveals brain networks for auditory language comprehension. Neuroimage 49, 3187–3197. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saygin, Z. M., Norton, E. S., Osher, D. E., Beach, S. D., Cyr, A. B., Ozernov-Palchik, O., et al. (2013). Tracking the roots of reading ability: white matter volume and integrity correlate with phonological awareness in prereading and early-reading kindergarten children. J. Neurosci. 33, 13251–13258. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4383-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaeffer, D. J., Krafft, C. E., Schwarz, N. F., Chi, L., Rodrigue, A. L., Pierce, J. E., et al. (2014). The relationship between uncinate fasciculus white matter integrity and verbal memory proficiency in children. Neuroreport 25, 921–925. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherf, K. S., Thomas, C., Doyle, J., and Behrmann, M. (2014). Emerging structure-function relations in the developing face processing system. Cereb. Cortex 24, 2964–2980. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmahmann, J. D., and Pandya, D. N. (2006). Fiber Pathways of the Brain. New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Schmahmann, J. D., Pandya, D. N., Wang, R., Dai, G., D’Arceuil, H. E., de Crespigny, A. J., et al. (2007). Association fibre pathways of the brain: parallel observations from diffusion spectrum imaging and autoradiography. Brain 130, 630–653. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnur, T. T., Schwartz, M. F., erg, D. Y., Hirshorn, E., Coslett, H. B., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). Localizing interference during naming: convergent neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence for the function of Broca’s area. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 322–327. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805874106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulze, K., Vargha-Khadem, F., and Mishkin, M. (2012). Test of a motor theory of long-term auditory memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 109, 7121–7125. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1204717109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, M. F., Faseyitan, O., Kim, J., and Coslett, H. B. (2012). The dorsal stream contribution to phonological retrieval in object naming. Brain 135, 3799–3814. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selnes, O. A., van Zijl, P. C. M., Barker, P. B., Hillis, A. E., and Mori, S. (2002). MR diffusion tensor imaging documented arcuate fasciculus lesion in a patient with normal repetition performance. Aphasiology 16, 897–902. doi: 10.1080/02687030244000374

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seltzer, B., and Pandya, D. N. (1984). Further observations on parieto-temporal connections in the rhesus monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 55, 301–312. doi: 10.1007/BF00237280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serra, L., Cercignani, M., Basile, B., Spanò, B., Perri, R., Fadda, L., et al. (2012). White matter damage along the uncinate fasciculus contributes to cognitive decline in AD and DLB. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 9, 326–333. doi: 10.2174/156720512800107555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shinoura, N., Midorikawa, A., Onodera, T., Tsukada, M., Yamada, R., Tabei, Y., et al. (2013). Damage to the left ventral, arcuate fasciculus and superior longitudinal fasciculus-related pathways induces deficits in object naming, phonological language function and writing, respectively. Int. J. Neurosci. 123, 494–502. doi: 10.3109/00207454.2013.765420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shuren, J. E., Schefft, B. K., Yeh, H.-S., Privitera, M. D., Cahill, W. T., and Houston, W. (1995). Repetition and the arcuate fasciculus. J. Neurol. 242, 596–598. doi: 10.1007/BF00868813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sierpowska, J., Gabarrós, A., Fernandez-Coello, A., Camins, à., Castañer, S., Juncadella, M., et al. (2015). Morphological derivation overflow as a result of disruption of the left frontal aslant white matter tract. Brain Lang. 142, 54–64. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sierpowska, J., Gabarrós, A., Fernández-Coello, A., Camins, à., Castañer, S., Juncadella, M., et al. (2019). White-matter pathways and semantic processing: intrasurgical and lesion-symptom mapping evidence. Neuroimage Clin. 22:101704. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sierpowska, J., Gabarrós, A., Fernandez-Coello, A., Camins, à., Castañer, S., Juncadella, M., et al. (2017). Words are not enough: nonword repetition as an indicator of arcuate fasciculus integrity during brain tumor resection. J. Neurosurg. 126, 435–445. doi: 10.3171/2016.2.JNS151592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sihvonen, A. J., Ripollés, P., Leo, V., Saunavaara, J., Parkkola, R., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., et al. (2021). Vocal music listening enhances post-stroke language network reorganization. eNeuro 8:ENEURO.0158-21.2021. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0158-21.2021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, G., and Citterio, A. (2017). Hemispheric asymmetries in dorsal language pathway white-matter tracts: a magnetic resonance imaging tractography and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroradiol. J. 30, 470–476. doi: 10.1177/1971400917720829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skudlarski, P., Jagannathan, K., Calhoun, V. D., Hampson, M., Skudlarska, B. A., and Pearlson, G. (2008). Measuring brain connectivity: diffusion tensor imaging validates resting state temporal correlations. Neuroimage 43, 554–561. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spinelli, E. G., Caso, F., Agosta, F., Gambina, G., Magnani, G., Canu, E., et al. (2015). A multimodal neuroimaging study of a case of crossed nonfluent/agrammatic primary progressive aphasia. J. Neurol. 262, 2336–2345. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7845-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sreedharan, R. M., Menon, A. C., James, J. S., Kesavadas, C., and Thomas, S. V. (2015). Arcuate fasciculus laterality by diffusion tensor imaging correlates with language laterality by functional MRI in preadolescent children. Neuroradiology 57, 291–297. doi: 10.1007/s00234-014-1469-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stamatakis, E. A., Shafto, M. A., Williams, G., Tam, P., and Tyler, L. K. (2011). White matter changes and word finding failures with increasing age. PLoS One 6:e14496. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stark, B. C., Basilakos, A., Hickok, G., Rorden, C., Bonilha, L., and Fridriksson, J. (2019). Neural organization of speech production: a lesion-based study of error patterns in connected speech. Cortex 117, 228–246. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.02.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinbrink, C., Vogt, K., Kastrup, A., Müller, H. P., Juengling, F. D., Kassubek, J., et al. (2008). The contribution of white and gray matter differences to developmental dyslexia: insights from DTI and VBM at 3.0T. Neuropsychologia 46, 3170–3178. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Su, M., Zhao, J., de Schotten, M. T., Zhou, W., Gong, G., Ramus, F., et al. (2018a). Alterations in white matter pathways underlying phonological and morphological processing in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 31, 11–19. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Su, M., de Schotten, M. T., Zhao, J., Song, S., Zhou, W., Gong, G., et al. (2018b). Vocabulary growth rate from preschool to school-age years is reflected in the connectivity of the arcuate fasciculus in 14-year-old children. Dev. Sci. 21:e12647. doi: 10.1111/desc.12647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Surbeck, W., Hänggi, J., Scholtes, F., Viher, P. V., Schmidt, A., Stegmayer, K., et al. (2020). Anatomical integrity within the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus and semantic processing deficits in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr. Res. 218, 267–275. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tak, H. J., and Jang, S. H. (2014). Relation between aphasia and arcuate fasciculus in chronic stroke patients. BMC Neurol. 14:46. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-14-46

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanabe, H., Sawada, T., Inoue, N., Ogawa, M., Kuriyama, Y., and Shiraishi, J. (1987). Conduction aphasia and arcuate fasciculus. Acta Neurol. Scand. 76, 422–427. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1987.tb03597.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tavor, I., Yablonski, M., Mezer, A., Rom, S., Assaf, Y., and Yovel, G. (2014). Separate parts of occipito-temporal white matter fibers are associated with recognition of faces and places. Neuroimage 86, 123–130. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teichmann, M., Rosso, C., Martini, J. B., Bloch, I., Brugières, P., Duffau, H., et al. (2015). A cortical-subcortical syntax pathway linking Broca’s area and the striatum. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 2270–2283. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tetzloff, K. A., Duffy, J. R., Clark, H. M., Strand, E. A., Machulda, M. M., Schwarz, C. G., et al. (2018). Longitudinal structural and molecular neuroimaging in agrammatic primary progressive aphasia. Brain 141, 302–317. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teubner-Rhodes, S., Vaden, K. I., Cute, S. L., Yeatman, J. D., Dougherty, R. F., and Eckert, M. A. (2016). Aging-resilient associations between the arcuate fasciculus and vocabulary knowledge: microstructure or morphology? J. Neurosci. 36, 7210–7222. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4342-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Cohen, L., Amemiya, E., Braga, L. W., and Dehaene, S. (2014). Learning to read improves the structure of the arcuate fasciculus. Cereb. Cortex 24, 989–995. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Dell’Acqua, F., Valabregue, R., and Catani, M. (2012). Monkey to human comparative anatomy of the frontal lobe association tracts. Cortex 48, 82–96. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Ffytche, D. H., Bizzi, A., Dell’Acqua, F., Allin, M., Walshe, M., et al. (2011). Atlasing location, asymmetry and inter-subject variability of white matter tracts in the human brain with MR diffusion tractography. Neuroimage 54, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, C., Avidan, G., Humphreys, K., Jung, K.-j., Gao, F., and Behrmann, M. (2009). Reduced structural connectivity in ventral visual cortex in congenital prosopagnosia. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 29–31. doi: 10.1038/nn.2224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, C., Avram, A., Pierpaoli, C., and Baker, C. (2015). Diffusion MRI properties of the human uncinate fasciculus correlate with the ability to learn visual associations. Cortex 72, 65–78. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, C., Moya, L., Avidan, G., Humphreys, K., Jung, K. J., Peterson, M. A., et al. (2008). Reduction in white matter connectivity, revealed by diffusion tensor imaging, may account for age-related changes in face perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 268–284. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson-Schill, S. L., and Botvinick, M. M. (2006). Resolving conflict: a response to Martin and Cheng (2006). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13, 402–408. doi: 10.3758/bf03193860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson-Schill, S. L., D’Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., and Farah, M. J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 94, 14792–14797. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.26.14792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson-Schill, S. L., Swick, D., Farah, M. J., D’Esposito, M., Kan, I. P., and Knight, R. T. (1998). Verb generation in patients with focal frontal lesions: a neuropsychological test of neuroimaging findings. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 95, 15855–15860. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.26.15855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toba, M. N., Migliaccio, R., Batrancourt, B., Bourlon, C., Duret, C., Pradat-Diehl, P., et al. (2018). Common brain networks for distinct deficits in visual neglect. A combined structural and tractography MRI approach. Neuropsychologia 115, 167–178. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Travers, B. G., Adluru, N., Ennis, C., Tromp do, P. M., Destiche, D., Doran, S., et al. (2012). Diffusion tensor imaging in autism spectrum disorder: a review. Autism Res. 5, 289–313. doi: 10.1002/aur.1243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Travis, K. E., Adams, J. N., Kovachy, V. N., Ben-Shachar, M., and Feldman, H. M. (2017). White matter properties differ in 6-year old readers and pre-readers. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 1685–1703. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1302-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, P., and Dick, A. S. (2016). Broca and Wernicke are dead, or moving past the classic model of language neurobiology. Brain Lang. 162, 60–71. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, P., and Gracco, V. L. (2009). Contribution of the pre-SMA to the production of words and non-speech oral motor gestures, as revealed by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS). Brain Res. 1268, 112–124. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, P., Perron, M., Deschamps, I., Kennedy-Higgins, D., Houde, J. C., Dick, A. S., et al. (2019). The role of the arcuate and middle longitudinal fasciculi in speech perception in noise in adulthood. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 226–241. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, P., and Small, S. (2011). Motor response selection in overt sentence production: a functional MRI study. Front. Psychol. 2:253. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tröstl, J., Sladky, R., Hummer, A., Kraus, C., Moser, E., Kasper, S., et al. (2011). Reduced connectivity in the uncinate fiber tract between the frontal cortex and limbic subcortical areas in social phobia. Eur. Psychiatry 26, 182–182. doi: 10.1016/S0924-9338(11)71893-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tseng, C. J., Froudist-Walsh, S., Kroll, J., Karolis, V., Brittain, P. J., Palamin, N., et al. (2019). Verbal fluency is affected by altered brain lateralization in adults who were born very preterm. eNeuro 6:ENEURO.0274-18.2018. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0274-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tu, S., Leyton, C. E., Hodges, J. R., Piguet, O., and Hornberger, M. (2016). Divergent longitudinal propagation of white matter degradation in logopenic and semantic variants of primary progressive aphasia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 49, 853–861. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuncer, M. S., Salvati, L. F., Grittner, U., Hardt, J., Schilling, R., Bährend, I., et al. (2021). Towards a tractography-based risk stratification model for language area associated gliomas. Neuroimage Clin. 29:102541. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turken, A., and Dronkers, N. (2011). The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:1. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ullsperger, M., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). Subprocesses of performance monitoring: a dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs. Neuroimage 14, 1387–1401. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urbanski, M., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Rodrigo, S., Oppenheim, C., Touzé, E., Méder, J. F., et al. (2011). DTI-MR tractography of white matter damage in stroke patients with neglect. Exp. Brain Res. 208, 491–505. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2496-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urger, S. E., De Bellis, M. D., Hooper, S. R., Woolley, D. P., Chen, S. D., and Provenzale, J. (2015). The superior longitudinal fasciculus in typically developing children and adolescents: diffusion tensor imaging and neuropsychological correlates. J. Child Neurol. 30, 9–20. doi: 10.1177/0883073813520503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdés-Sosa, M., Bobes, M. A., Quiñones, I., Garcia, L., Valdes-Hernandez, P. A., Iturria, Y., et al. (2011). Covert face recognition without the fusiform-temporal pathways. Neuroimage 57, 1162–1176. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallesi, A., and Babcock, L. (2020). Asymmetry of the frontal aslant tract is associated with lexical decision. Brain Struct. Funct. 225, 1009–1017. doi: 10.1007/s00429-020-02054-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Wouwe, N. C., Pallavaram, S., Phibbs, F. T., Martinez-Ramirez, D., Neimat, J. S., Dawant, B. M., et al. (2017). Focused stimulation of dorsal subthalamic nucleus improves reactive inhibitory control of action impulses. Neuropsychologia 99, 37–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderauwera, J., De Vos, A., Forkel, S. J., Catani, M., Wouters, J., Vandermosten, M., et al. (2018). Neural organization of ventral white matter tracts parallels the initial steps of reading development: a DTI tractography study. Brain Lang. 183, 32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderauwera, J., Wouters, J., Vandermosten, M., and Ghesquière, P. (2017). Early dynamics of white matter deficits in children developing dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandermosten, M., Boets, B., Wouters, J., and Ghesquière, P. (2012). A qualitative and quantitative review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in reading and dyslexia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1532–1552. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandermosten, M., Vanderauwera, J., Theys, C., De Vos, A., Vanvooren, S., Sunaert, S., et al. (2015). A DTI tractography study in pre-readers at risk for dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaquero, L., Ramos-Escobar, N., Cucurell, D., François, C., Putkinen, V., Segura, E., et al. (2021). Arcuate fasciculus architecture is associated with individual differences in pre-attentive detection of unpredicted music changes. Neuroimage 229:117759. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varriano, F., Pascual-Diaz, S., and Prats-Galino, A. (2018). When the FAT goes wide: right extended frontal aslant tract volume predicts performance on working memory tasks in healthy humans. PLoS One 13:e0200786. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varriano, F., Pascual-Diaz, S., and Prats-Galino, A. (2020). Distinct components in the right extended frontal aslant tract mediate language and working memory performance: a tractography-informed VBM study. Front. Neuroanat. 14:21. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2020.00021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vassal, F., Boutet, C., Lemaire, J. J., and Nuti, C. (2014). New insights into the functional significance of the frontal aslant tract: an anatomo-functional study using intraoperative electrical stimulations combined with diffusion tensor imaging-based fiber tracking. Br. J. Neurosurg. 28, 685–687. doi: 10.3109/02688697.2014.889810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verly, M., Gerrits, R., Sleurs, C., Lagae, L., Sunaert, S., Zink, I., et al. (2019). The mis-wired language network in children with developmental language disorder: insights from DTI tractography. Brain Imag. Behav. 13, 973–984. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9903-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vernooij, M. W., Smits, M., Wielopolski, P. A., Houston, G. C., Krestin, G. P., and van der Lugt, A. (2007). Fiber density asymmetry of the arcuate fasciculus in relation to functional hemispheric language lateralization in both right- and left-handed healthy subjects: a combined fMRI and DTI study. Neuroimage 35, 1064–1076. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidorreta, J. G., Garcia, R., Moritz-Gasser, S., and Duffau, H. (2011). Double dissociation between syntactic gender and picture naming processing: a brain stimulation mapping study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 331–340. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visser, M., Jefferies, E., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010). Semantic processing in the anterior temporal lobes: a meta-analysis of the functional neuroimaging literature. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1083–1094. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Der Heide, R. J., Skipper, L. M., Klobusicky, E., and Olson, I. R. (2013). Dissecting the uncinate fasciculus: disorders, controversies and a hypothesis. Brain 136, 1692–1707. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, A. D., Paré-Blagoev, E. J., Clark, J., and Poldrack, R. A. (2001). Recovering meaning: left prefrontal cortex guides controlled semantic retrieval. Neuron 31, 329–338. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00359-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wakana, S., Caprihan, A., Panzenboeck, M. M., Fallon, J. H., Perry, M., Gollub, R. L., et al. (2007). Reproducibility of quantitative tractography methods applied to cerebral white matter. Neuroimage 36, 630–644. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wakana, S., Jiang, H., Nagae-Poetscher, L. M., van Zijl, P. C., and Mori, S. (2004). Fiber tract-based atlas of human white matter anatomy. Radiology 230, 77–87. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2301021640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, G. M., Schwartz, M. F., Kimberg, D. Y., Faseyitan, O., Brecher, A., Dell, G. S., et al. (2011). Support for anterior temporal involvement in semantic error production in aphasia: new evidence from VLSM. Brain Lang. 117, 110–122. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Fernández-Miranda, J. C., Verstynen, T., Pathak, S., Schneider, W., and Yeh, F.-C. (2013). Rethinking the role of the middle longitudinal fascicle in language and auditory pathways. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2347–2356. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, K., Li, X., Huang, R., Ding, J., Song, L., and Han, Z. (2020). The left inferior longitudinal fasciculus supports orthographic processing: evidence from a lesion-behavior mapping analysis. Brain Lang. 201:104721. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2019.104721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Pathak, S., Stefaneanu, L., Yeh, F. C., Li, S., and Fernandez-Miranda, J. C. (2016). Subcomponents and connectivity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus in the human brain. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 2075–2092. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1028-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warker, J. A., Xu, Y., Dell, G. S., and Fisher, C. (2009). Speech errors reflect the phonotactic constraints in recently spoken syllables, but not in recently heard syllables. Cognition 112, 81–96. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wedeen, V. J., Wang, R. P., Schmahmann, J. D., Benner, T., Tseng, W. Y. I., Dai, G., et al. (2008). Diffusion spectrum magnetic resonance imaging (DSI) tractography of crossing fibers. Neuroimage 41, 1267–1277. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiller, C., Reisert, M., Peto, I., Hennig, J., Makris, N., Petrides, M., et al. (2021). The ventral pathway of the human brain: a continuous association tract system. Neuroimage 234:117977. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, K. S., Yeatman, J. D., and Wandell, B. A. (2017). The posterior arcuate fasciculus and the vertical occipital fasciculus. Cortex 97, 274–276. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welcome, S. E., and Joanisse, M. F. (2014). Individual differences in white matter anatomy predict dissociable components of reading skill in adults. Neuroimage 96, 261–275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wernicke, C., Hahn, E., and Sachs, H. (1897). Photographischer Atlas de Gehirns: Schniktte durch das menschliche gehirn in photographischen originalen. Abteilung I—32 frontalschnitte durch eine grosshirn- hemisphaü re. Breslau: Schletter’schen Buchhandlung (Franck and Weigert).

Whitwell, J. L., Avula, R., Senjem, M. L., Kantarci, K., Weigand, S. D., Samikoglu, A., et al. (2010). Gray and white matter water diffusion in the syndromic variants of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 74, 1279–1287. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181d9edde

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiecki, T. V., and Frank, M. J. (2013). A computational model of inhibitory control in frontal cortex and basal ganglia. Psychol. Rev. 120, 329–355. doi: 10.1037/a0031542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, S. M., Dronkers, N. F., Ogar, J. M., Jang, J., Growdon, M. E., Agosta, F., et al. (2010). Neural correlates of syntactic processing in the nonfluent variant of primary progressive aphasia. J. Neurosci. 30, 16845–16854. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2547-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, S. M., Galantucci, S., Tartaglia, M. C., Rising, K., Patterson, D. K., Henry, M. L., et al. (2011). Syntactic processing depends on dorsal language tracts. Neuron 72, 397–403. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, S. M., and Saygın, A. P. (2004). Grammaticality judgment in aphasia: deficits are not specific to syntactic structures, aphasic syndromes, or lesion sites. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 238–252. doi: 10.1162/089892904322984535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, S. M., Yen, M., and Eriksson, D. K. (2018). An adaptive semantic matching paradigm for reliable and valid language mapping in individuals with aphasia. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 3285–3307. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, F. C. K., Chandrasekaran, B., Garibaldi, K., and Wong, P. C. M. (2011). White matter anisotropy in the ventral language pathway predicts sound-to-word learning success. J. Neurosci. 31, 8780–8785. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0999-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Y., Sun, D., Wang, Y., and Wang, Y. (2016). Subcomponents and Connectivity of the Inferior Fronto-Occipital Fasciculus Revealed by Diffusion Spectrum Imaging Fiber Tracking. Front. Neuroanat. 10:88. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2016.00088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yablonski, M., Menashe, B., and Ben-Shachar, M. (2021). A general role for ventral white matter pathways in morphological processing: going beyond reading. Neuroimage 226:117577. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamada, K., Nagakane, Y., Mizuno, T., Hosomi, A., Nakagawa, M., and Nishimura, T. (2007). MR tractography depicting damage to the arcuate fasciculus in a patient with conduction aphasia. Neurology 68:789. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000256348.65744.b2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, M., Li, Y., Li, J., Yao, D., Liao, W., and Chen, H. (2017). Beyond the arcuate fasciculus: damage to ventral and dorsal language pathways in aphasia. Brain Topogr. 30, 249–256. doi: 10.1007/s10548-016-0503-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeatman, J. D., Dougherty, R. F., Ben-Shachar, M., and Wandell, B. A. (2012). Development of white matter and reading skills. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 109, E3045–E3053. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206792109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeatman, J. D., Dougherty, R. F., Rykhlevskaia, E., Sherbondy, A. J., Deutsch, G. K., Wandell, B. A., et al. (2011). Anatomical properties of the arcuate fasciculus predict phonological and reading skills in children. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3304–3317. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeatman, J. D., Weiner, K. S., Pestilli, F., Rokem, A., Mezer, A., and Wandell, B. A. (2014). The vertical occipital fasciculus: a century of controversy resolved by in vivo measurements. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 111, E5214–E5223. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418503111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeterian, E. H., Pandya, D. N., Tomaiuolo, F., and Petrides, M. (2012). The cortical connectivity of the prefrontal cortex in the monkey brain. Cortex 48, 58–81. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, J. S., Morshed, R. A., Mansoori, Z., Cha, S., and Berger, M. S. (2020). Disruption of frontal aslant tract is not associated with long-term postoperative language deficits. World Neurosurg. 133, 192–195. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2019.09.128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yue, Q., Martin, R. C., Hamilton, A. C., and Rose, N. S. (2018). Non-perceptual regions in the left inferior parietal lobe support phonological short-term memory: evidence for a buffer account? Cereb. Cortex 29, 1398–1413. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zemmoura, I., Burkhardt, E., and Herbet, G. (2021). The inferior longitudinal fasciculus: anatomy, function and surgical considerations. J. Neurosurg. Sci. 65, 590–604. doi: 10.23736/S0390-5616.21.05391-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, B., Chang, J., Park, J., Tan, Z., Tang, L., Lyu, T., et al. (2021). Uncinate fasciculus and its cortical terminals in aphasia after subcortical stroke: a multi-modal MRI study. Neuroimage Clin. 30:102597. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Gao, J., Shi, H., Huang, B., Wang, X., Situ, W., et al. (2014). Sex differences of uncinate fasciculus structural connectivity in individuals with conduct disorder. Biomed. Res. Int. 2014:673165. doi: 10.1155/2014/673165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Wei, X., Xie, S., Zhou, Z., Shang, D., Ji, R., et al. (2018). Multifunctional roles of the ventral stream in language models: advanced segmental quantification in post-stroke aphasic patients. Front. Neurol. 9:89. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Zhang, J., Oishi, K., Faria, A. V., Jiang, H., Li, X., et al. (2010). Atlas-guided tract reconstruction for automated and comprehensive examination of the white matter anatomy. Neuroimage 52, 1289–1301. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, J., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Altarelli, I., Dubois, J., and Ramus, F. (2016). Altered hemispheric lateralization of white matter pathways in developmental dyslexia: evidence from spherical deconvolution tractography. Cortex 76, 51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuurbier, L. A., Nikolova, Y. S., Åhs, F., and Hariri, A. R. (2013). Uncinate fasciculus fractional anisotropy correlates with typical use of reappraisal in women but not men. Emotion 13, 385–390. doi: 10.1037/a0031163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: language, white matter, dorsal and ventral pathways, inferior longitudinal fasciculus, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, uncinate fasciculus, arcuate fasciculus, frontal aslant tract

Citation: Shekari E and Nozari N (2023) A narrative review of the anatomy and function of the white matter tracts in language production and comprehension. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1139292. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1139292

Received: 06 January 2023; Accepted: 24 February 2023;
Published: 27 March 2023.

Edited by:

Lars Kuchinke, International Psychoanalytic University Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Jacqueline Cummine, University of Alberta, Canada
Heidi M. Feldman, Stanford University, United States

Copyright © 2023 Shekari and Nozari. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nazbanou Nozari, bnozari@andrew.cmu.edu

Download